Cardiovascular disease and distribution of cognitive function in elderly people: the Rotterdam study
BMJ 1994; 308 doi: https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.308.6944.1604 (Published 18 June 1994) Cite this as: BMJ 1994;308:1604- M M B Breteler,
- J J Claus,
- D E Grobbee,
- A Hofman
- Department of Epidemiology and Biostatistics and of Neurology, Erasmus University Medical School, Rotterdam, Netherlands
- Correspondence to: Dr Breteler.
- Accepted 22 March 1994
abstract
Objective: To investigate the distribution of cognitive function in elderly people and to assess the impact of clinical manifestations of atherosclerotic disease on this distribution.
Design: Single centre population based cross sectional door to door study.
Setting: Ommoord, a suburb of Torrerdam, the Netherlands.
Subjects: 4971 subjects aged 55 to 94 years.
Main outcome measure: Cognitive function as measured by the mini mental state examination.
Results: The overall participation rate in the study was 80%. Cognitive test data were available for 90% of the participants. Increasing age and lower educational level were associated with poorer cognitive function. Previous vascular events, presence of plaques in the carotid arteries, and presence of peripheral arterial atherosclerotic disease were associated with worse cognitive performance independent of the effects of age and education. On average the differences were moderate; however, they reflected the net result of a shift of the total population distribution of cognitive function towards lower values. Thereby, they resulted in a considerable increase in the proportion of subjects with scores indicative of dementia.
Conclusions: These findings are compatible with the view that atherosclerotic disease accounts for considerable cognitive impairment in the general population.
Clinical implications
Clinical implications
An increasing number of people are suffering from demonia or cognitive decline yet effective treatment it still lacking
Intervention on vascular causes of dementia may be fearble
This study suggests that cognitive declist-itemne is not synonymous with age but is associated with age related diseases
Atherosclerosis results in a considerable increase in the proportion of cognitively impaired subjects in the population
The more rational approach to prevent cognitive decline resulting from vascular causes is by mass intervention
Introduction
The incidence and severity of cognitive impairment and dementia strongly increase with age. Dementia is often labelled an age related disorder with the implicit suggestion that the decline in cognitive function may be intrinsic to aging itself. Many old people, however, show no mental decline at all.1 In those instances when cognitive decline does occur, in general it develops insidiously and asymptomatically before it reaches the clinical threshold at which it becomes manifest and is referred to as dementia. The division of populations into diseased versus normal tends to neglect this heterogeneity.2,3 It has therefore been suggested that research in degenerative disorders should focus not only on disease but also on the total distribution of health states.4 We present data on the distribution of cognitive function and on the impact of clinical manifestations of atherosclerotic disease on this distribution in a geographically defined sample of subjects aged 55 to 94 years.
Methods
Study population
The Rotterdam study is a single centre prospective follow up study for which the total population aged 55 years or over, including institutionalised people, of the suburb of Ommoord in Rotterdam, the Netherlands, is studied. The study has been approved by the medical ethics committee of Erasmus University. Written informed consent is obtained from all participants. Rationale and design of the study have been described elsewhere.5 In short, the objective of the study is to investigate determinants of chronic and disabling cardiovascular, neurodegenerative, locomotor, and ophthalmological diseases. Independently living participants are extensively interviewed at home and are subsequently clinically examined during two visits to a research centre. For institutionalised people all examinations were performed in their institute. Enrolment in the study started in June 1990 and was based on random selection procedures. By 31 December 1992, 7120 residents of Ommoord had been invited and 5673 subjects had actually participated. The overall participation rate was 80%, similar for men and women. To guarantee adequate numbers for both sexes and all ages we confined our sample for this analysis to those aged 55 to 94 years, which left 5527 subjects. Of these, cognitive test data were available for 4971 (90%) and these data were included in the present analyses.
Measurements
As part of the screening protocol for dementia, cognitive function of all participants was tested with the mini mental state examination during the first visit to the research centre.6 The examination contain 18 items that cover orientation, memory, attention, ability to follow commands, and copying a complex figure and yields a maximum score of 30 points.7 The test was administered by specially trained assistants.
Attained level of education was assessed by classifying formal schooling according to the standard classification of education used by the Netherlands Central Bureau of Statistics, which is comparable with the International Standard Classification of Education (UNESCO, Paris, 1976).8 For the present analyses subjects were grouped into those with at most primary education, those with junior vocational training, and those with senior vocational or academic training.
A history of stroke was assessed by direct questioning. For a diagnosis of stroke symptoms should have been present for at least 24 hours, and confirmation of the diagnosis by a treating physician was required. Presence of abnormalities that would suggest previous myocardial infarction were assessed on a 12 lead electrocardiogram by study physicians according to preset criteria with any suspected abnormality reviewed by a cardiologist. Atherosclerosis of the arteries of the lower legs and of the carotid arteries was non-invasively assessed with the use of Doppler and ultrasound. The ratio of the ankle to brachial systolic blood pressure (ankle-brachial index) is thought to reflect the presence of atherosclerotic abnormalities of the arterial walls in the lower extremities and has been shown to be a good indicator of generalised atherosclerosis.9 Ankle systolic blood pressure was determined with the subject in supine position at both right and left posterior tibial arteries with a Doppler ultrasound transducer by using a random zero sphygmomanometer (cuff size 38 x 14 cm). Arterial disease was considered present when the left or right ankle-brachial index was less than 0.90.10,11 Ultrasonography of both carotid arteries was performed with a 7.5 MHz linear array transducer and a duplex scanner (ATL UltraMark IV, Advanced Technology Laboratories, Bethel, Western Australia). Presence of atherosclerotic lesions, defined as a focal widening relative to adjacent segments with protrusion into the lumen, was assessed in the internal carotid arteries.
With respect to smoking behaviour subjects were categorised in groups of current smokers, former smokers, and those who had never smoked.
Analysis
The total distribution of cognitive function in the population is presented graphically according to age, level of education, gender, vascular events, and indicators of atherosclerosis. For each of these characteristics the distribution of the mini mental state examination score among subjects with the characteristic was obtained by direct standardisation to the distribution of results in a reference population.12 The reference populations for the distributions according to age, gender, and education were subjects aged 55 to 64 years, men, and lowest educational level, respectively. The reference populations for the distributions according to vascular events and atherosclerosis were subjects without the characteristic of interest. Distributions were standardised for age and gender.
In addition, we presented the distributions numerically. For each characteristic the mean score on the examination was calculated for those with and without the characteristic of interest. Because the distributions of scores were highly skewed to the left we further characterised them by calculating centile scores (5th, 25th, 50th, 75th, 95th).
An important question is how differences in scores should be interpreted. On an individual level a difference of, for example, one point cannot be considered clinically relevant and could easily be explained by intrapatient variability. An average difference of one point on a poulation level, however, may have important clinical consequences. We substantiated the clinical relevance of the differences between distributions by calculating for each distribution the proportion of people who scored below cutoffs that have been recommended as suspect for dementia (scores of 247 and 2613). The statistical significance of the difference between proportions scoring below the cutoff was assessed by multivariate logistic regression. Possible confounding by smoking was evaluated by adding this as a covariate (ever versus never; current versus never) to the multiple logistic regression models.
Although the overall response was reasonably high, the effective response rate was lower among older than younger subjects. Response rates were also higher among older subjects who were institutionalised than among their independently living contemporaries. To assess whether these differences could have biased our overall results we conducted additional analyses. Firstly, we evaluated whether the relations differed between younger (age 55 to 74 years) and older (age 75 to 94 years) subjects. Secondly, we compared the results in the oldest age group for institutionalised people with those for non-institutionalised people.
Results
Table I presents the age and gender distribution of the study population and the age specific participation rate and proportions of participants for whom cognitive test data were available. Total non-response increased with age as did the number of subjects who completed the interview but did not come to the research centre. Of the 4971 subjects for whom mini mental state examination scores were available, 1939 (39%) had attended only primary school or less, 1690 (34%) had junior vocational training, and 1342 (27%) had senior vocational training or academic training. Of this group, 249 (5%) had had a stroke, 348 (7%) had had myocardial infarction according to electrocardiographic readings, 1044 (21%) had peripheral arterial disease, and 1790 (36%) had plaques in the internal carotid arteries on either or both sides.
Figure 1 depicts the population distribution of scores according to age, gender, and education. The upper panel shows that with increasing age the distributions shifted toward lower values whereas their skew increased, reflecting increasing variability. The distributions for men and women overlapped almost completely, in particular when education was taken into account (fig 1, middle panel). The distributions by education are shown in the lower panel. Subjects with higher education performed better, as shown by a total shift of their distributions towards higher scores, and with less variability.
Table II further quantifies these distributions. The differences in mean scores across age and education and between men and women reflect the average shift of the distributions relative to that of their reference group. For subjects aged 85 to 94 years compared with subjects aged 55 to 64 years it was 5.0 points, for highly educated people compared with people with at most primary school education it was 1.7 points, and for women compared with men it was 0.2 points. The variability in the distributions is reflected in the range between the 5th and the 95th centile; the larger this range the more variation exists between people. In all instances a shift of the distribution towards lower values was accompanied by an increase in the variability, and the combined effect thereof was a considerable increase in the proportion of people scoring below the cutoffs.
Figure 2 compares the cognitive performance among subjects with and without a previous vascular event. Both a history of stroke and electrocardiographic evidence of a previous myocardial infarction were associated with a shift of the population distribution of scores toward lower values. A similar pattern was observed for presence versus absence of atherosclerotic disease, either localised in the carotid arteries or in the large vessels of the lower extremities (fig 3). Table III gives the quantitiative data regarding these distributions. As can be seen from the range between the 5th and 95th centiles of the distributions shifts of the total distribution towards lower values were again accompanied by an increase in variability. Correspondingly, the proportion of subjects scoring below the cutoffs of 24 or 26 on the mini mental state examination increased.
When we included current or ever smoking in the multiple logistic regression models with which we compared subjects below and above the cutoffs on the examination, the odds ratios for the various vascular determinants barely changed, suggesting that there was no confounding in our data due to smoking.
Subanalyses of the results among younger versus older subjects and among institutionalised versus non-institutionalised people did not show any substantial differences.
Discussion
We presented the distributions of cognitive function in a geographically defined population of subjects aged 55 to 94 years according to age, gender, and education and the influence of clinical manifestations of atherosclerotic disease on these distributions. We found that for subjects who were older, less well educated, and had vascular and atherosclerotic disease the distributions were shifted toward lower values and the variability was increased whereas gender had almost no effect on the localisation or shape of the distribution.
Before discussing our findings, we must consider whether non-response may have influenced our results. The overall participation rate in our study was high (80%) and so was the overall availability of cognitive test results (90%). Furthermore, institutionalised people were included in our study and the participation rate among them was high (83%). Although high response rates diminish the possibility of serious distortion of the study results, among independently living people non-response increased with age, and part of this non-response was probably selective and related to physical or mental morbidity. The associations that we observed among older subjects, however, were similar to those among younger subjects for whom response rates were highest. Furthermore, the results were similar for institutionalised and independently living people. This suggests that the differences in effective response rates according to age did not introduce additional bias. We think the most plausible pattern of selective non-response is that it increased with increasing impairment particularly when there was a combination of physical and mental handicaps. Therefore, we consider it most likely that if selective non-response has biased our findings it resulted in an underestimation of the strength of the relations that we investigated.
The negative associations between age and cognitive performance and between education and cognitive test performance are well recognised.*RF 14-16* Most previous studies presented summary results. One study of 365 people presented information on the total distribution of cognitive function and concluded that with less education the distribution was shifted downwards but similarly shaped.17,18 Our results confirmed the previous reports but showed in addition that with a decrease in average performance the shape of the distributions changed owing to increasing variability among individual people. With regard to age an explanation for this finding could be that cognitive decline is not intrinsic to aging but rather that age is a proxy for accumulated lifetime exposures affecting cognitive function.
One putative risk factor for cognitive decline is vascular disease, in particular atherosclerosis.19,20 We found that previous vascular events and presence of atherosclerosis were associated with a shift of the total population distribution towards lower levels. The mean difference, however, was less than one point on the mini mental state examination, which raises the question of whether this is clinically relevant. The answer is not to be found on the individual level but rather on the population level. The mean difference reflects the aggregate experience of people with a large decline and of those with no decline at all. This study does not indicate which people suffered cognitive impairment as a result of vascular disease. These findings, however, are compatible with the view that on a population level atherosclerotic disease can account for considerable cognitive impairment as reflected by the shifts and change in shape of the distribution and the increasing proportion of subjects scoring below a specified cutoff. In most cases vascular events such as stroke and myocardial infarction reflect a near end stage of atherosclerotic disease. Therefore, it is not surprising that these were associated with a larger population shift in cognitive performance than mere presence of atherosclerosis. It should be noted that, whereas stroke and myocardial infarction were present in 5% and 7% of the population, respectively, one fifth to one third of the population had evidence for atherosclerosis in the peripheral or carotid arteries. This suggests that the total impact of atherosclerosis on the amount of cognitive impairment in the population at large may be much greater than that contributable to severe atherosclerosis resulting in clinically overt disease.
A question that remains to be answered is whether the prevention of atherosclerosis would result in the otherwise affected subjects assuming the distribution of the now unaffected people. If so this could mean a major health benefit. Although the average gain per person would be small, the population gain might be substantial.4 It seems timely to conduct a study to investigate whether intervention on risk factors for atherosclerosis can prevent cognitive decline on a population level.
We thank all coworkers of the Rotterdam study for their enthusiastic and skilful contributions to the data collection and management, in particular Henriette Ensing, Els van Helden, Eline Herist, and Marion Huuklsloot. The study was supported in part by the NESTOR programme for geriatric research (Ministry of Health and Ministry of Education), the Netherlands Heart Foundation, the Netherlands Organisation for Scientific Research (NWO), and the Municipality of Rotterdam.