iBet uBet web content aggregator. Adding the entire web to your favor.
iBet uBet web content aggregator. Adding the entire web to your favor.



Link to original content: https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3656
Object-based attention involves the sequential activation of feature-specific cortical modules | Nature Neuroscience
Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Article
  • Published:

Object-based attention involves the sequential activation of feature-specific cortical modules

Subjects

Abstract

Object-based theories of attention propose that the selection of an object's feature leads to the rapid selection of all other constituent features, even those that are task irrelevant. We used magnetoencephalographic recordings to examine the timing and sequencing of neural activity patterns in feature-specific cortical areas as human subjects performed an object-based attention task. Subjects attended to one of two superimposed moving dot arrays that were perceived as transparent surfaces on the basis either of color or speed of motion. When surface motion was attended, the magnetoencephalographic waveforms showed enhanced activity in the motion-specific cortical area starting at 150 ms after motion onset, followed after 60 ms by enhanced activity in the color-specific area. When surface color was attended, this temporal sequence was reversed. This rapid sequential activation of the relevant and irrelevant feature modules provides a neural basis for the binding of an object's features into a unitary perceptual experience.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Figure 1: Stimuli for experiments 1 and 2.
Figure 2: The sensory effects of color and motion.
Figure 3: Effect of attention on motion processing in color-absent conditions.
Figure 4: Experiment 1: sequential selection of attended motion feature and unattended color feature.
Figure 5: Experiment 2: sequential selection of attended color feature and unattended motion feature.
Figure 6: Source waveforms showing timing of sensory effects and relevant and irrelevant feature selections.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Posner, M.I., Snyder, C.R.R. & Davidson, B.J. Attention and the detection of signals. J. Exp. Psychol. 109, 160–174 (1980).

    Google Scholar 

  2. Wolfe, J.M. Guided search 2.0 A revised model of visual search. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 1, 202–238 (1994).

    Google Scholar 

  3. Chen, Z. Object-based attention: a tutorial review. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 74, 784–802 (2012).

    Google Scholar 

  4. Scholl, B.J. Objects and attention: the state of the art. Cognition 80, 1–46 (2001).

    Google Scholar 

  5. Boehler, C.N., Schoenfeld, M.A., Heinze, H.-J. & Hopf, J.-M. Object-based selection of irrelevant features is not confined to the attended object. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 2231–2239 (2011).

    Google Scholar 

  6. Hopf, J.M., Heinze, H.J., Schoenfeld, M.A. & Hillyard, S.A. Spatio-temporal analysis of visual attention. in The Cognitive Neurosciences IV (ed. Gazzaniga, M.S.) 235–250 (MIT Press, Cambgridge, Massachusetts, 2009).

  7. Desimone, R. & Duncan, J. Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 18, 193–222 (1995).

    Google Scholar 

  8. Duncan, J. Selective attention and the organization of visual information. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 113, 501–517 (1984).

    Google Scholar 

  9. Duncan, J., Humphreys, G.W. & Ward, R.M. Competitive brain activity in visual attention. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 7, 255–261 (1997).

    Google Scholar 

  10. Roelfsema, P.R. Cortical algorithms for perceptual grouping. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 29, 203–227 (2006).

    Google Scholar 

  11. O'Craven, K.M., Downing, P.E. & Kanwisher, N. fMRI evidence for objects as the units of attentional selection. Nature 401, 584–587 (1999).

    Google Scholar 

  12. Schoenfeld, M.A. et al. Dynamics of feature binding during object-selective attention. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 100, 11806–11811 (2003).

    Google Scholar 

  13. He, Z.J. & Nakayama, K. Visual attention to surfaces in three-dimensional space. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 92, 11155–11159 (1995).

    Google Scholar 

  14. Valdes-Sosa, M., Cobo, A. & Pinilla, T. Transparent motion and object-based attention. Cognition 66, B13–B23 (1998).

    Google Scholar 

  15. Valdes-Sosa, M., Cobo, A. & Pinilla, T. Attention to object files defined by transparent motion. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 26, 488–505 (2000).

    Google Scholar 

  16. Schoenfeld, M.A. et al. Spatio-temporal analysis of feature-based attention. Cereb. Cortex 17, 2468–2477 (2007).

    Google Scholar 

  17. Rodríguez, V., Valdes-Sosa, M. & Freiwald, W. Dividing attention between form and motion during transparent surface perception. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 13, 187–193 (2002).

    Google Scholar 

  18. Duncan, J. in Attention and Performance XVI (ed. McClelland, J.L.) 549–578 (MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, 1996).

  19. Singer, W. Neuronal synchrony: a versatile code for the definition of relations? Neuron 24, 49–65, 111–125 (1999).

    Google Scholar 

  20. Palanca, B.J. & DeAngelis, G.C. Does neuronal synchrony underlie visual feature grouping? Neuron 46, 333–346 (2005).

    Google Scholar 

  21. Hadjikhani, N., Liu, A.K., Dale, A.M., Cavanagh, P. & Tootell, R.B. Retinotopy and color sensitivity in human visual cortical area V8. Nat. Neurosci. 1, 235–241 (1998).

    Google Scholar 

  22. Zeki, S., McKeefry, D.J., Bartels, A. & Frackowiak, R.S. Has a new color area been discovered? Nat. Neurosci. 1, 335–336 (1998).

    Google Scholar 

  23. Seymour, K., Clifford, C.W., Logothetis, N.K. & Bartels, A. The coding of color, motion and their conjunction in the human visual cortex. Curr. Biol. 19, 177–183 (2009).

    Google Scholar 

  24. Stoppel, C.M. et al. Feature-based attention modulates direction-selective hemodynamic activity within human MT. Hum. Brain Mapp. 32, 2183–2192 (2011).

    Google Scholar 

  25. Katzner, S., Busse, L. & Treue, S. Attention to the color of a moving stimulus modulates motion-signal processing in macaque area MT: evidence for a unified attentional system. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 3, 12 (2009).

    Google Scholar 

  26. Wannig, A., Rodriguez, V. & Freiwald, W.A. Attention to surfaces modulates motion processing in extrastriate area MT. Neuron 54, 639–651 (2007).

    Google Scholar 

  27. Treisman, A. Feature binding, attention and object perception. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 353, 1295–1306 (1998).

    Google Scholar 

  28. Egly, R., Driver, J. & Rafal, R.D. Shifting visual attention between objects and locations: evidence from normal and parietal lesion subjects. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 123, 161–177 (1994).

    Google Scholar 

  29. Goldsmith, M. & Yeari, M. Central-cue discriminability modulates object-based attention by influencing spatial attention. Exp. Psychol. 59, 132–137 (2012).

    Google Scholar 

  30. Law, M.B. & Abrams, R.A. Object-based selection within and beyond the focus of spatial attention. Percept. Psychophys. 64, 1017–1027 (2002).

    Google Scholar 

  31. He, X., Fan, S., Zhou, K. & Chen, L. Cue validity and object-based attention. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 1085–1097 (2004).

    Google Scholar 

  32. Kasai, T. Attention-spreading based on hierarchical spatial representations for connected objects. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22, 12–22 (2010).

    Google Scholar 

  33. Martínez, A., Teder-Salejarvi, W. & Hillyard, S.A. Spatial attention facilitates selection of illusory objects: evidence from event-related brain potentials. Brain Res. 1139, 143–152 (2007).

    Google Scholar 

  34. Martínez, A. et al. Objects are highlighted by spatial attention. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 18, 298–310 (2006).

    Google Scholar 

  35. Muller, N.G. & Kleinschmidt, A. Dynamic interaction of object- and space-based attention in retinotopic visual areas. J. Neurosci. 23, 9812–9816 (2003).

    Google Scholar 

  36. Zinni, M.M.A. & Hillyard, S.A. The neural basis of color binding to an attended object. in Electrophysiology of Attention: Signals of the Mind (ed. Mangun, G.R.) (Elsevier, New York, 2013).

  37. Sohn, W., Papathomas, T.V., Blaser, E. & Vidnyanszky, Z. Object-based cross-feature attentional modulation from color to motion. Vision Res. 44, 1437–1443 (2004).

    Google Scholar 

  38. Khoe, W., Mitchell, J.F., Reynolds, J.H. & Hillyard, S.A. Exogenous attentional selection of transparent superimposed surfaces modulates early event-related potentials. Vision Res. 45, 3004–3014 (2005).

    Google Scholar 

  39. Reynolds, J.H., Alborzian, S. & Stoner, G.R. Exogenously cued attention triggers competitive selection of surfaces. Vision Res. 43, 59–66 (2003).

    Google Scholar 

  40. Guthrie, D. & Buchwald, J.S. Significance testing of difference potentials. Psychophysiology 28, 240–244 (1991).

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank M. Wachter for help with the data collection and N. Nönnig and M. Scholz for technical support. This work was supported by grants Scho1217/1-2 and SFB779 TP A1 from the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG) and by grants from the US National Science Foundation (BCS-1029084) and the National Institute of Mental Health (1P50MH86385).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

M.A.S. designed and performed the experiments, analyzed the data and wrote the paper. J.-M.H. designed the experiments and analyzed the data. C.M. analyzed the data. H.-J.H. supervised the performance of the experiments and data analysis. S.A.H. designed the experiments, supervised the performance of the experiments and data analysis, and wrote the paper.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Mircea A Schoenfeld.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

Integrated supplementary information

Supplementary Figure 1 Source activity waveforms from experiment 1

The two traces at the top show the averaged source activity waveforms for motion sensitive sources in the middle occipital gyri (red tracing) and color sensitive sources in inferior occipital cortex (blue tracing) from Experiment 1, where selection of the surface was based on motion speed and color was the task-irrelevant feature. The subsequent four source activity waveforms (red tracings) originate from left and right motion-sensitive sources in the middle occipital gyri obtained from the analysis of the differences formed by subtracting the ERF on grey-slow*/red-fast trials from the ERF on grey-slow/red-fast* trials (first two red waveforms) and grey-fast/red-slow* trials from ERFs on grey-fast*/ red-slow trials (last two red waveforms). The bottom four source activity waveforms (blue tracings) originate from left and right color-sensitive sources in the inferior occipital cortex obtained from the differences formed by subtracting the ERF on grey-slow*/red-fast trials from the ERF on grey-slow/red-fast* trials (first two blue waveforms) and grey-fast/red-slow* trials from ERFs on grey-fast*/ red-slow trials (last two blue waveforms). Note that all waveform deflections are positive (upwards) regardless of the sign of the subtractions, since these waveforms depict positive current density measures.

Supplementary Figure 2 Source activity waveforms from experiment 2

The two traces at the top show the averaged source activity waveforms for color sensitive sources in inferior occipital cortex (blue tracing) and motion sensitive sources in the middle occipital gyri (red tracing) from Experiment 2, where selection of the surface was based on color and motion was the task-irrelevant feature. The subsequent four source activity waveforms (blue tracings) originate from left and right color-sensitive sources in the inferior occipital cortex obtained from the differences formed by subtracting the grey-slow*/red-fast trials from ERFs on grey-slow/red-fast* trials (first two blue waveforms) and subtracting the grey-fast*/red-slow trials from ERFs on grey-fast/red-slow* (last two blue waveforms). The bottom four source activity waveforms (red tracings) originate from left and right motion-sensitive sources in the middle occipital gyri obtained from the differences formed by subtracting the grey-slow*/red-fast trials from ERFs on grey-slow/red-fast* trials (first two red waveforms) and subtracting the grey-fast*/red-slow trials from ERFs on grey-fast/red-slow* (last two red waveforms).

Supplementary information

Supplementary Text and Figures

Supplementary Figures 1 and 2 (PDF 1420 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Schoenfeld, M., Hopf, JM., Merkel, C. et al. Object-based attention involves the sequential activation of feature-specific cortical modules. Nat Neurosci 17, 619–624 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3656

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3656

This article is cited by

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing