Abstract
Cognitive load theory has been designed to provide guidelines intended to assist in the presentation of information in a manner that encourages learner activities that optimize intellectual performance. The theory assumes a limited capacity working memory that includes partially independent subcomponents to deal with auditory/verbal material and visual/2- or 3-dimensional information as well as an effectively unlimited long-term memory, holding schemas that vary in their degree of automation. These structures and functions of human cognitive architecture have been used to design a variety of novel instructional procedures based on the assumption that working memory load should be reduced and schema construction encouraged. This paper reviews the theory and the instructional designs generated by it.
Similar content being viewed by others
REFERENCE
Aasman, J., Mulder, G., and Mulder, L. J. M. (1987). Operator effort and the measurement of heart-rate variability. Human Fact. 29: 161–170.
Anderson, J. R., Boyle, C. F., Corbell, A., and Lewis, M. (1990). Cognitive modelling and intelligent tutoring. Artif. Intell. 42: 7–49.
Ayres, P. (1993). Why goal-free problems can facilitate learning. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 18: 376–381.
Baddeley, A. (1992). Working memory. Science 255: 556–559.
Barfield, W. (1986). Expert-novice differences for software: Implications for problem solving and knowledge acquisition. Behav. Inform. Technol. 5: 15–29.
Bobis, J., Sweller, J., and Cooper, M. (1993). Cognitive load effects in a primary school geometry task. Learn. Instruct. 3: 1–21.
Bobis, J., Sweller, J., and Cooper, M. (1994). Demands imposed on primary-school students by geometric models. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 19: 108–117.
Borg, G. (1978). Subjective aspects of physical work. Ergonomics 21: 215–220.
Bratfisch, O., Borg, G., and Dornic, S. (1972). Perceived Item-Difficulty in Three Tests of Intellectual Performance Capacity. Report No. 29, Institute of Applied Psychology, Stockholm, Sweden.
Carroll, J. M. (1990). The Nurnberg Funnel: Designing Minimalist Instruction for Practical Computer Skill, MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.
Carroll, W. (1994). Using worked examples as an instructional support in the algebra classroom. J. Educ. Psychol. 86: 360–367.
Cerpa, N., Chandler, P., and Sweller, J. (1996). Some conditions under which integrated computer-based training software can facilitate learning. J. Educ. Comput. Res. 15: 345–367.
Chandler, P., and Sweller, J. (1991). Cognitive load theory and the format of instruction. Cognit. Instr. 8: 293–332.
Chandler, P., and Sweller, J. (1992). The split-attention effect as a factor in the design of instruction. Brit. J. Educ. Psychol. 62: 233–246.
Chandler, P., and Sweller, J. (1996). Cognitive load while learning to use a computer program. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 10: 151–170.
Chase, W. G., and Simon, H. A. (1973). Perception in chess. Cognit. Psychol. 4: 55–81.
Chi, M., Bassok, M., Lewis, M., Reimann, P., and Glaser, R. (1989). Self-explanations: How students study and use examples in problem solving. Cognit. Sci. 13: 145–182.
Chi, M., Glaser, R., and Rees, E. (1982). Expertise in problem solving. In Sternberg, R. (ed.), Advances in the Psychology of Human Intelligence, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 7–75.
Cooper, G., and Sweller, J. (1987). The effects of schema acquisition and rule automation on mathematical problem-solving transfer. J. Educ. Psychol. 79: 347–362.
Cormier, S. M. and Hagman, J. D. (eds.) (1987). Transfer of learning: Contemporary research and applications, Academic Press, San Diego, CA.
De Groot, A. D. (1966). Perception and memory versus thought: Some old ideas and recent findings. In Kleinmuntz, B. (ed.), Problem Solving, Wiley, New York.
Egan, D. E., and Schwartz, B. J. (1979). Chunking in recall of symbolic drawings. Mem. Cognit. 7: 149–158.
Ericsson, K. A., and Charness, N. (1994). Expert performance: Its structure and acquisition. Am. Psychol. 49: 725–747.
Gopher, D., and Braune, R. (1984). On the psychophysics of workload: Why bother with subjective measures? Human Fact. 26: 519–532.
Hendy, C. H., Hamilton, K. M., and Landry, L. N. (1993). Measuring subjective workload: When is one scale better than many? Human Fact. 35: 579–601.
Jeffries, R., Turner, A., Polson, P., and Atwood, M. (1981). Processes involved in designing software. In Anderson, J. R. (ed.), Cognitive Skills and Their Acquisition, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 255–283.
Jelsma, O., and Bijlstra, J. P. (1988). Training for Transfer in Learning to Detect, Diagnose, and Compensate System Failures. Proceedings of the Seventh European Annual Conference on Human Decision Making and Manual Control, Paris, France, pp. 256–262.
Jelsma, O., and Bijlstra, J. P. (1990). PROCESS: Program for research on operator control in an experimental simulated setting. IEEE Trans. Syst. Man, Cybernet. 20: 1221–1228.
Jelsma, O., and van Merrienboer, J. J. G. (1989). Contextual interference: Interactions with reflection-impulsivity. Percept. Motor Skills 68: 1055–1064.
Jelsma, O., van Merrienboer, J. J. G. and Bijlstra, J. P. (1990). The ADAPT design model: Towards instructional control of transfer. Instr. Sci. 19: 89–120.
Kalyuga, S., Chandler, P., and Sweller, J. (1998). Levels of expertise and instructional design. Human Fact. 40: 1–17.
Kotovsky, K., Hayes, J. R., and Simon, H. A. (1985). Why are some problems hard? Evidence from Tower of Hanoi. Cognit. Psychol. 17: 248–294.
Lazonder, A. W., and Van der Meij, H. (1993). The minimal manual: Is less really more? Int. J. Man Machine Stud. 39: 729–752.
LeFevre, J., and Dixon, P. (1986). Do written instructions need examples? Cognit. Instr. 3(1): 1–30.
Lieberman, H. (1986). An example-based environment for beginning programmers. Instr. Sci. 14: 277–292.
Marcus, N., Cooper, M., and Sweller, J. (1996). Understand instructions. J. Educ. Psychol. 88: 49–63.
Mayer, R. E., and Anderson, R. B. (1991). Animations need narrations: An experimental test of dual-coding hypothesis. J. Educ. Psychol. 83: 484–490.
Mayer, R. E., and Anderson, R. B. (1992). The instructive animation: Helping students build connections between words and pictures in multimedia learning. J. Educ. Psychol. 84: 444–452.
Mayer, R., Bove, W., Bryman, A., Mars, R., and Tapangco, L. (1996). When less is more: Meaningful learning from visual and verbal summaries of science textbook lessons. J. Educ. Psychol. 88: 64–73.
McNamara, D., Kintsch, E., Songer, N. B., and Kintsch, W. (1996). Are good texts always better? Interactions of text coherence, background knowledge, and levels of understanding in learning from text. Cognit. Instr. 14: 1–43.
Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychol. Rev. 63: 81–97.
Miller, W. (1937). The picture crutch in reading. Elem. Eng. Rev. 14: 263–264.
Mousavi, S. Y., Low, R., and Sweller, J. (1995). Reducing cognitive load by mixing auditory and visual presentation modes. J. Educ. Psychol. 87: 319–334.
Mulder, L. J. M. (1988). Assessment of Cardiovascular Reactivity by Means of Spectral Analysis. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands.
Mwangi, W., and Sweller, J. (in press). Learning to solve compare word problems: The effect of example format and generating self-explanations. Cognit. Instr.
Owen, E., and Sweller, J. (1985). What do students learn while solving mathematics problems? J. Educ. Psychol. 77: 272–284.
Paas, F. G. W. C. (1992). Training strategies for attaining transfer of problem-solving skill in statistics: A cognitive load approach. J. Educ. Psychol. 84: 429–434.
Paas, F. G. W. C., and van Merrienboer, J. J. G. (1993). The efficiency of instructional conditions: An approach to combine mental-effort and performance measures. Human Fact. 35: 737–743.
Pass, F. G. W. C., and van Merrienboer, J. J. G. (1994a). Variability of worked examples and transfer of geometrical problem solving skills: A cognitive load approach. J. Educ. Psychol. 86: 122–133.
Paas, F. G. W. C., and van Merrienboer, J. J. G. (1994b). Instructional control of cognitive load in the training of complex cognitive tasks. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 6: 351–371.
Paas, F. G. W. C., van Merrienboer, J. J. G., and Adam, J. J. (1994). Measurement of cognitive load in instructional research. Percept. Motor Skills 79: 419–430.
Penney, C. (1989). Modality effects and the structure of short-term verbal memory. Mem. Cognit. 17: 398–422.
Pirolli, P. L. (1991). Effects of examples and their explanations in a lesson on recursion: A production system analysis. Cognit. Instr. 8: 207–259.
Pirolli, P. L. and Anderson, J. R. (1985). The role of learning from examples in the acquisition of recursive programming skills. Can. J. Psychol. 3: 240–272.
Quilici, J. L., and Mayer, R. E. (1996). Role of examples in how students learn to categorize statistics word problems. J. Educ. Psychol. 88: 144–161.
Reder, L., and Anderson, J. R. (1980). A comparison of texts and their summaries: Memorial consequences. J. Verb. Learn. Verb. Behav. 19: 121–134.
Reder, L., and Anderson, J. R. (1982). Effects of spacing and embellishment on memory for main points of a text. Mem. Cognit. 10: 97–102.
Schneider, W., and Shiffrin, R. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: I. Detection, search and attention. Psychol. Rev. 84: 1–66.
Schooler, J., and Engstler-Schooler, L. (1990). Verbal overshadowing of visual memories: Some things are better left unsaid. Cognit. Psychol. 22: 36–71.
Segal, J., and Ahmad, K. (1993). The role of examples in the leaching of programming languages. J. Educ. Comput. Res. 9: 115–129.
Shiffrin, R., and Schneider, W. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending, and a general theory. Psychol. Rev. 84: 127–190.
Simon, H., and Gilmartin, K. (1973). A simulation of memory for chess positions. Cognit. Psychol. 5: 29–46.
Singley, M. K., and Anderson, J. R. (eds.) (1989). The Transfer of Cognitive Skill, Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.
Smith, S. M., Ward, T. B., and Schumacher, I. S. (1993). Constraining effects of examples in a creative generation task. Mem. Cognit. 21: 837–845.
Solman, R., Singh, N., and Kehoe, E. J. (1992). Pictures block the learning of sightwords. Educ. Psychol. 12: 143–153.
Sweller, J. (1988). Cognitive load during problem solving: Effects on learning. Cognit. Sci. 12: 257–285.
Sweller, J. (1994). Cognitive load theory, learning difficulty, and instructional design. Learn. Instr. 4: 295–312.
Sweller, J., and Chandler, P. (1994). Why some material is difficult to learn. Cognit. Instr. 12: 185–233.
Sweller, J., and Cooper, G. A. (1985). The use of worked examples as a substitute for problem solving in learning algebra. Cognit. Instr. 2: 59–89.
Sweller, J., and Levine, M. (1982). Effects of goal specificity on means-ends analysis and learning. J. Exp. Psychol: Learn., Mem., Cognit. 8: 463–474.
Sweller, J., Mawer, R., and Ward, M. (1983). Development of expertise in mathematical problem solving. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 112: 634–656.
Sweller, J., Chandler, P., Tierney, P., and Cooper, M. (1990). Cognitive load and selective attention as factors in the structuring of technical material. J. Exp. Psychol.: Gen. 119: 176–192.
Tarmizi, R., and Sweller, J. (1988). Guidance during mathematical problem solving. J. Educ. Psychol. 80: 424–436.
Tindall-Ford, S., Chandler, P., and Sweller, J. (1997). When two sensory modes are better than one. J. Exp. Psychol.: Appl. 3: 257–287.
Trafton, J. G., and Reiser, B. J. (1993). The Contribution of Studying Examples and Solving Problems to Skill Acquisition. Proceedings of the 15th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 1017–1022.
Van Merrienboer, J. J. G. (1990). Strategies for programming instruction in high school: Program completion vs. program generation. J. Educ. Comput. Res. 6: 265–287.
Van Merrienboer, J. J. G. (1992). Training strategies for teaching introductory computer programming. In Engel, F. L., Bouwhuis, D. G., Bssser, T., and d'Ydewalle, G. (eds.), Cognitive Modelling and Interactive Environments in Language Learning, Springer Verlag, Berlin, pp. 81–88.
Van Merrienboer, J. J. G. (1997). Training Complex Cognitive Skills: A Four-Component Instructional Design Model for Technical Training, Educational Technology Publications, Englewood Cliffs, NJ.
Van Merrienboer, J. J. G., and De Croock, M. B. M. (1992). Strategies for computer-based programming instruction: Program completion vs. program generation. J. Educ. Comput. Res. 8: 365–394.
Van Merrienboer, J. J. G, de Croock, M. B. M., and Jelsma, O. (1997). The transfer paradox: Effects of contextual interference on retention and transfer performance in learning a complex cognitive skill. Percept. Motor Skills 84: 784–786.
Van Merrienboer, J. J. G., and Dijkstra, S. (1997). The four-component instructional design model for training complex cognitive skills. In Tennyson, R. D., and Schott, F. (eds.), Instructional Design: Theory and Research (Vol. 1), Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.
Van Merrienboer, J. J. G., Jelsma, O., and Paas, F. G. W. C. (1992). Training for reflective expertise: A four-component instructional design model for training complex cognitive skills. Educ. Technol. Res. Devel. 40(2): 23–43.
Van Merrienboer, J. J. G., and Krammer, H. P. M. (1987). Instructional strategies and tactics for the design of introductory computer programming courses in high school. Instr. Sci. 16: 251–285.
Van Merrienboer, J. J. G., and Krammer, H. P. M. (1990). The “completion strategy” in programming instruction: Theoretical and empirical support. In Dijkstra, S., van Hout-Wolters, B. H. M., and van der Sijde, P. C. (eds.), Research on Instruction, Educational Technology Publications, Englewood Cliffs, NJ, pp. 45–61.
Van Merrienboer, J. J. G., Krammer, H. P. M., and Maaswinkel, R. M. (1994). Automating the planning and construction of programming assignments for teaching introductory computer programming. In Tennyson, R. D. (ed.), Automating Instructional Design, Development, and Delivery (NATO ASI Series F, Vol. 119), Springer Verlag, Berlin, pp. 61–77.
Van Merrienboer, J. J. G., and Luursema, J. J. (1995). Implementing instructional models in computer-based learning environments: A case study in problem selection. In Liao, T. T. (ed.), Advanced Educational Technology: Research Issues and Future Potential (NATO ASI Series F, Vol. 145). Springer Verlag, Berlin.
Van Merrienboer, J. J. G., Luursema, J. J., Kingma, H., Houweling, F., and de Vries, A. P. (1995). Fuzzy logic instructional models: The dynamic construction of programming assignments in CASCO. In Tennyson, R. D., and Barron, A. E. B. (eds.), Automating. Instructional Design: Computer-Based Development and Delivery Tools (NATO ASI Series F), Springer Verlag, Berlin.
Van Merrienboer, J. J. G., and Paas, F. G. W. C. (1990). Automation and schema acquisition in learning elementary compute programming: Implications for the design of practice. Comput. Human Behav. 6: 273–289.
Vollmeyer, R., Burns, B., and Holyoak, K. (1996). The impact of goal specificity on strategy use and the acquisition of problem structure. Cognit. Sci. 20: 75–100.
Ward, M., and Sweller, J. (1990). Structuring effective worked examples. Cognit. Instr. 7: 1–39.
Wierwille, W. W., and Eggemeier, F. L. (1993). Recommendations for mental workload measurement in a test and evaluation environment. Human Fact. 35: 263–281.
Yeung, A., Jin, P., and Sweller, J. (1998). Cognitive load and learner expertise: Split-attention and redundancy effects in reading with explanatory notes. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 23: 1–21.
Zhu, X., and Simon, H. (1987). Learning mathematics from examples and by doing. Cognit. Instr. 4: 137–166.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Sweller, J., van Merrienboer, J.J.G. & Paas, F.G.W.C. Cognitive Architecture and Instructional Design. Educational Psychology Review 10, 251–296 (1998). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1022193728205
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1022193728205