Abstract
In two experiments, we transferred perceptual load theory to the dynamic field of team sports and tested the predictions derived from the theory using a novel task and stimuli. We tested a group of college students (N = 33) and a group of expert team sport players (N = 32) on a general perceptual load task and a complex, soccer-specific perceptual load task in order to extend the understanding of the applicability of perceptual load theory and further investigate whether distractor interference may differ between the groups, as the sport-specific processing task may not exhaust the processing capacity of the expert participants. In both, the general and the specific task, the pattern of results supported perceptual load theory and demonstrates that the predictions of the theory also transfer to more complex, unstructured situations. Further, perceptual load was the only determinant of distractor processing, as we neither found expertise effects in the general perceptual load task nor the sport-specific task. We discuss the heuristic utility of using response-competition paradigms for studying both general and domain-specific perceptual-cognitive adaptations.
Explore related subjects
Discover the latest articles, news and stories from top researchers in related subjects.References
Allard F, Graham S, Paarsalu ML (1980) Perception in sport: basketball. J Sport Psychol 2:14–21
Bavelier D, Deruelle C, Proksch J (2000) Positive and negative compatibility effects. Percept Psychophys 62:100–112
Bavelier D, Dye MWG, Hauser P (2006) Do deaf individuals see better? Trends Cogn Sci 10:512–518
Beck DM, Lavie N (2005) Look here but ignore what you see: effects of distractors at fixation. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 31:592–607
Broadbent DE (1958) Perception and communication. Oxford University Press, New York
Cañal-Bruland R, Lotz S, Hagemann N, Schorer J, Strauss B (2011) Visual span and change detection in soccer: an expertise study. J Cogn Psychol 23:302–310
Chaddock L, Neider MB, Voss MW, Gaspar JG, Kramer AF (2011) Do athletes excel at everyday tasks? Med Sci Sports Exerc 43:1920–1926
Chase WG, Simon HA (1973) Perception in chess. Cogn Psychol 4:55–81
Cosman JD, Vecera S (2012) Object-based attention overrides perceptual load to modulate visual distraction. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 38:576–579
De Groot AD (1965) Thought and choice in chess. Mouton, The Hague
Deutsch JA, Deutsch D (1963) Attention: some theoretical considerations. Psychol Rev 70:80–90
Didierjean A, Marmèche E (2005) Anticipatory representation of visual basketball scenes by novice and expert players. Vis Cogn 12:265–283
Duncan J (1980) The locus of interference in the perception of simultaneous stimuli. Psychol Rev 87:272–300
Eccles DW (2006) Thinking outside of the box: the role of environmental adaptation in the acquisition of skilled and expert performance. J Sports Sci 24:1103–1114
Ericsson KA, Krampe R, Tesch-Römer C (1993) The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychol Rev 100:363–406
Ericsson KA, Charness N, Feltovich P, Hoffman RR (2006) Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge
Eriksen CW (1995) The flankers task and response competition: a useful tool for investigating a variety of cognitive problems. Vis Cogn 2:101–118
Fiedler K (2011) Voodoo correlations are everywhere–not only in neuroscience. Perspect Psychol Sci 6:163–171
Forster S, Lavie N (2007) High perceptual load makes everybody equal: eliminating individual differences in distractibility with load. Psychol Sci 18:377–381
Forster S, Lavie N (2008) Failures to ignore entirely irrelevant distractors: the role of load. J Exp Psychol Appl 14:73–83
Forster S, Lavie N (2009) Harnessing the wandering mind: the role of perceptual load. Cognition 111:345–355
Furley P, Memmert D (2011) Studying cognitive adaptations in the field of sport: broad or narrow transfer? A comment on Allen, Fioratou, and McGeorge (2011). Percept Mot Skills 113(2):481–488
Garland DJ, Barry JR (1991) Cognitive advantage in sport: the nature of perceptual structures. Am J Psychol 104:211–228
Gibson BS, Bryant TA (2008) The identity intrusion effect: attentional capture or perceptual load? Vis Cogn 16:182–199
Gorman AD, Abernethy B, Farrow D (2011) Investigating the anticipatory nature of pattern perception in sport. Mem Cogn 39:894–901
Green CS, Bavelier D (2003) Action video game modifies visual selective attention. Nature 42:534–537
Handy TC, Mangun GR (2000) Attention and spatial selection: electrophysiological evidence for modulation by perceptual load. Percept Psychophys 62:175–186
Huang-Pollock CL, Carr TH, Nigg JT (2002) Development of selective attention: perceptual load influences early versus late attentional selection in children and adults. Dev Psychol 38:363–375
Kingston A, Smilek D, Ristic J, Friesen CK, Eastwood JD (2003) Attention, Researchers! It is time to take a look at the real world. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 12:176–180
Lachter J, Forster KI, Ruthruff E (2004) Forty-five years after Broadbent (1958): still no identification without attention. Psychol Rev 111:880–913
Lavie N (1995) Perceptual load as a necessary condition for selective attention. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 21:451–468
Lavie N (2005) Distracted and confused?: selective attention under load. Trends Cogn Sci 9:75–82
Lavie N (2010) Attention, distraction, and cognitive control under load. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 19:143–148
Lavie N, Fox E (2000) The role of perceptual load in negative priming. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 26:1038–1052
Lavie N, Tsal Y (1994) Perceptual load as a major determinant of the locus of selection in visual attention. Percept Psychophys 56:183–197
Lavie N, Lin Z, Zokaei N, Thoma V (2009) The role of perceptual load in object recognition. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 35:1346–1358
MacLeod CM (1991) Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. Psychol Bull 109:163–203
Maylor E, Lavie N (1998) The influence of perceptual load on age differences in selective attention. Psychol Aging 13:563–573
Meiser T (2011) Much pain, little gain? Paradigm-specific models and methods in experimental psychology. Perspect Psychol Sci 6:183–191
Moray N (1959) Attention in dichotic listening: affective cues and the influence of instructions. Q J Exp Psychol 11:56–60
Paquet L, Craig GL (1997) Evidence for selective target processing with a low perceptual load flankers task. Mem Cogn 25:182–189
Posner MI (1978) Chronometric explorations of mind. Erlbaum, Hillsdale
Rees G, Frith C, Lavie N (2001) Processing of irrelevant visual motion during performance of an auditory attention task. Neuropsychologia 39:937–949
Reingold EM, Charness N, Pomplun M, Stampe DM (2001) Visual span expert chess players: evidence from eye movements. Psychol Sci 12:48–55
Remington A, Swettenham J, Campbell R, Coleman M (2009) Selective attention and perceptual load in autism spectrum disorder. Psychol Sci 20:1388–1393
Schmid S (2008) Erklärungskraft der Load-Theorie im Sport. Die Übertragbarkeit allgemeiner Flankierreizstimuli in den Bereich des Basketballs. (Explanatory power of the load theory in sports. Transfer of the general flaker paradigm to the dynamic field of basketball). Unpublished Master Thesis
Sedlmeier P, Hertwig R, Gigerenzer G (1998) Are judgments of the positional frequencies of letters systematically biased due to availability? J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 24:754–770
Shadish WR, Cook TD, Campbell DT (2002) Experimental and quasi-experimental designs for generalized causal inference. Houghton Mifflin, Boston
Simmons JP, Nelson LD, Simonsohn U (2011) False-positive psychology: undisclosed flexibility in data collection and analysis allows presenting anything as significant. Psychol Sci 22:1359–1366
Starkes JL (1987) Skill in field hockey: the nature of the cognitive advantage. J Sport Psychol 9:146–160
Starkes JL, Helsen WF, Jack R (2001) Expert performance in sport and dance. In: Singer RN, Hausenblas HA, Janelle CM (eds) Handbook of research in sport psychology. Wiley, New York, pp 174–201
Stroop JR (1935) Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J Exp Psychol 18:643–662
Tipper SP (1985) The negative priming effect: inhibitory priming by ignored objects. Q J Exp Psychol 37:571–590
Tversky A, Kahneman D (1973) Extensional versus intuitive reasoning: the conjunction fallacy in probability judgment. Psychol Rev 90:293–315
Vogel EK, Awh E (2008) How to exploit diversity for scientific gain: using individual differences to constrain cognitive theory. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 17:171–176
Voss MW, Kramer AF, Basak C, Prakash RS, Roberts B (2010) Are expert athletes ‘expert’ in the cognitive laboratory? A meta-analytic review of cognition and sport expertise. Appl Cogn Psychol 24:812–826
Williams AM, Hodges NJ, North JS, Barton G (2006) Perceiving patterns of play in dynamic sport tasks: identifying the essential information underlying skilled performance. Perception 35:317–332
Wood N, Cowan N (1995) The cocktail party phenomenon revisited: how frequent are attention shifts to one’s name in an irrelevant auditory channel? J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 21:255–260
Acknowledgments
Special thanks go to Bente Wegner for helping with the data collection in this study.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Furley, P., Memmert, D. & Schmid, S. Perceptual load in sport and the heuristic value of the perceptual load paradigm in examining expertise-related perceptual-cognitive adaptations. Cogn Process 14, 31–42 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-012-0529-x
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-012-0529-x