iBet uBet web content aggregator. Adding the entire web to your favor.
iBet uBet web content aggregator. Adding the entire web to your favor.



Link to original content: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01889762
A brief history of connectionism and its psychological implications | AI & SOCIETY Skip to main content
Log in

A brief history of connectionism and its psychological implications

  • Published:
AI & SOCIETY Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Critics of the computational connectionism of the last decade suggest that it shares undesirable features with earlier empiricist or associationist approaches, and with behaviourist theories of learning. To assess the accuracy of this charge the works of earlier writers are examined for the presence of such features, and brief accounts of those found are given for Herbert Spencer, William James and the learning theorists Thorndike, Pavlov and Hull. The idea that cognition depends on associative connections among large networks of neurons is indeed one with precedents, although the implications of this for psychological issues have been interpreted variously — not all versions of connectionism are alike.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Explore related subjects

Discover the latest articles, news and stories from top researchers in related subjects.

References

  • Anderson, J. A. and Rosenfeld, E. (eds) (1988).Neurocomputing: A Reader. MIT Press, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baernstein, H. D. and Hull, C. L. (1931). A mechanical model of the conditioned reflex.Journal of General Psychology, 5, 99–106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ballard, D. H. (1986). Cortical connections and parallel processing: structure and function.Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 9, 67–120.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boakes, R. A. (1984).From Darwin to Behaviourism. Cambridge University Press.

  • Cary, M. P. and Burish, T. G. (1988). Etiology and treatment of the psychological side-effects associated with cancer chemotherapy: a critical review and discussion.Psychological Bulletin, 104, 307–325.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crick, F. H. C. and Asanuma, C. (1986). Certain aspects of the anatomy and physiology of the cerebral cortex. In J. L. McClelland and D. E. Rumelhart, (eds),Parallel Distributed Processing. Volume 2. Psychological and Biological Models. MIT Press, London, 333–371.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davey, G. (ed.) (1987).Cognitive Processes and Pavlovian Conditioning in Humans. Wiley, Chichester.

    Google Scholar 

  • de la Mettrie, J. (1748/1912).Man a Machine. Open Court, La Salle.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dickinson, A. (1988). Intentionality in animal conditioning. In L. Weiskrantz (ed.),Thought without Language, Clarendon Press, Oxford, 305–325.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, H. J. (1952). The effects of psychotherapy: an evaluation.Journal of Consulting Psychology, 16, 319–324.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, H. J. (1986).The Decline and Fall of the Freudian Empire. Penguin, Harmondsworth.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. (1965). Could meaning be an Rm?Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 4, 73–81.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. (1983).The Modularity of Mind. MIT Press, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. (1987).Psychosemantics: The Problem of Meaning in the Philosophy of Mind. MIT Press, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. and Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1988). Connectionism and cognitive architecture: a critical analysis.Cognition, 28, 3–71.

    Google Scholar 

  • French, T. M. (1933). The interrelations between psychoanalysis and the experimental work of Pavlov.American Journal of Psychiatry, 12, 1165–1195.

    Google Scholar 

  • French, T. M. (1944). Clinical approaches to the dynamics of behaviour. In Hunt, J. McV. (ed.),Personality and the Behaviour Disorders. Vol. I. Ronald Press, New York, 255–268.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freud, S. (1936).The Problem of Anxiety. Norton, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hartley, D. (1749/1834).Observations on Man, his Frame, his Duty and his Expectations. Tegg, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hebb, D. O. (1949).The Organization of Behaviour. Chapman & Hall, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hilgard, E. R. and Bower, G. H. (1975).Theories of Learning 4th edition. Prentice-Hall, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hinton, G. E. and Anderson, J. A. (eds) (1981).Parallel Models of Associative Memory. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. (1930). Knowledge and purpose as habit mechanisms.Psychological Review, 37, 511–525.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. (1931). Goal attraction and directing ideas conceived as habit phenomena.Psychological Review, 38, 478–506.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. (1934). The concept of habit-family hierarchy and maze learning.Psychological Review, 41, Part I, 33–54; Part II, 134–152.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. (1937). Mind, mechanism and adaptive behaviour.Psychological Review, 44, 1–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. (1943).Principles of Behaviour. Appleton Century Crofts, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. (1952).A Behaviour System. Yale University Press, New Haven.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, J. H. (1888/1931).Selected Writings. Volume One, J. Taylor (ed.) Hodder and Stoughton, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1890/1983).The Principles of Psychology. Harvard University Press, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Konorski, J. (1948).Conditioned Reflexes and Neuron Organization. Cambridge University Press.

  • Konorski, J. (1967).Integrative Activity of the Brain. University of Chicago Press.

  • Krueger, R. C. and Hull, C. L. (1931). An electro-chemical parallel to the conditioned reflex.Journal of General Psychology, 5, 262–269.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewin, K. (1935).A Dynamic Theory of Personality. McGraw-Hill, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Livingstone, M. S. and Hubel, D. H. (1984). Anatomy and physiology of a colour system in the primate visual cortex.Journal of Neuroscience, 4, 309–356.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lynch, G. (1986).Synapses, Circuits and the Beginnings of Memory. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mackintosh, N. J. (1974).The Psychology of Animal Learning. Academic Press, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mackintosh, N. J. (1983).Conditioning and Associative Learning. Clarendon Press, Oxford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mackintosh, N. J. (1988). Approaches to the study of animal intelligence.British Journal of Psychology, 79, 509–525.

    Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, J. L., Rumelhart, D. E. and Hinton, G. E. (1986). The appeal of parallel distributed processing. In D. E. Rumelhart and J. L. McClelland (eds),Parallel Distributed Processing. Volume 1. Foundations. MIT Press, London, 3–44.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, N. E. (1944). Experimental studies of conflict. In J. McV. Hunt (ed.).Personality and the Behaviour Disorders. Vol. I. Ronald Press, New York, 431–465.

    Google Scholar 

  • Minsky, M. and Papert, S. (1969).Perceptrons, MIT Press, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mowrer, O. H. (1939). A stimulus-response analysis of anxiety and its role as a reinforcing agent.Psychological Review, 46, 553–565.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mowrer, O. H. (1940). Anxiety reduction and learning.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 27, 497–516.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mowrer, O. H. (1960).Learning Theory and the Symbolic Processes. Wiley, Chichester.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olton, D. S. (1979). Mazes, maps, and memory.American Psychologist, 34, 583–596.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osgood, C. E. (1953).Method and Theory in Experimental Psychology. Oxford University Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Papert, S. (1988). One AI or many?Daedelus, Winter, 1–14.

  • Pavlov, I. P. (1927).Conditioned Reflexes: An Investigation of the Physiological Activity of the Cerebral Cortex. Dover, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piatelli-Palmarini, M. (1989). Evolution and cognition: From “learning” to parameter setting in biology and the study of language.Cognition, 31, 1–44.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. and Prince, A. (1988). On language and connectionism.Cognition, 28, 73–193.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1985).Computation and Cognition, MIT Press, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rescorla, R. A. (1978). Some implications of a cognitive perspective on Pavlovian conditioning. In S. H. Hulse, H. Fowler and W. K. Honig (eds),Cognitive Processes in Animal Behaviour. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, London, 15–50.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rescorla, R. A. and Wagner, A. R. (1972). A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In A. H. Black and W. F. Prokasy (eds),Classical Conditioning II: current research and theory. Appleton Century Crofts, New York, 64–69.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenblatt, F. (1962).Principles of Neurodynamics. Spartan, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D. E. and McClelland, J. L. (1986a). PDP models and general issues in cognitive science. In D. E. Rumelhart and J. L. McClelland (eds),Parallel Distributed Processing. Volume 1. Foundations. MIT Press, London, 110–146.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D. E. and McClelland, J. L. (1986b). On learning the past tenses of English verbs. In J. L. McClelland and D. E. Rumelhart (eds),Parallel Distributed Processing. Volume 2. Psychological and Biological Models. MIT Press, London, 216–271.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D. E. and Zipser, D. (1986). Feature discovery by competitive learning. In D. E. Rumelhart and J. L. McClelland (eds),Parallel Distributed Processing. Volume 1. Foundations. MIT Press, London, 152–193.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D. E., Hinton, G. E. and Williams, R. J. (1986a). Learning internal representations by error propogation. In D. E. Rumelhart and J. L. McClelland (eds),Parallel Distributed Processing. Volume 1. Foundations. MIT Press, London, 318–364.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D. E., Smolensky, P., McClelland, J. L. and Hinton, G. E. (1986b). Schemata and sequential thought processes in PDP models. In J. L. McClelland, and D. E. Rumelhart (eds),Parallel Distributed Processing. Volume 2. Psychological and Biological Models. London: MIT Press, 7–57.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sears, R. R. (1944). Experimental analysis of psychoanalytic phenomena. In J. McV. Hunt (ed.),Personality and the Behaviour Disorders. Vol. I. Ronald Press, New York, 306–332.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sears, R. R., Maccoby, E. E. and Levin, H. (1957).Patterns of Child Rearing. Row and Peterson, Evanston IL.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sherrington, C. S. (1906).The Integrative Action of the Nervous System. Yale University Press, New Haven.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1953).Science and Human Behaviour. Macmillan, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1989). The origins of cognitive thought.American Psychologist, 44, 13–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smolensky, P. (1988). On the proper treatment of connectionism.Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 11, 1–74.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spence, K. W. (1952). Clark Leonard Hull: 1884–1952.American Journal of Psychology, 65, 639–646.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spencer, H. (1855/1899).Principles of Psychology: Vols I and II. Williams and Norgate, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stewart, J., de Wit, H. and Eikelbom, R. (1984). Role of unconditioned and conditioned drug effects in the self-administration of opiates and stimulants.Psychological Review, 91, 251–268.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stich, S. (1983).From Folk Psychology to Cognitive Science: The Case Against Belief. MIT Press, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strong, G. W. and Whitehead, B. A. (1989). A solution to the tag assignment problem of neural networks.Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 12, 381–433.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sulloway, F. J. (1979).Freud, Biologist of the Mind. Basic Books, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sutton, R. S. and Barto, A. G. (1981). Toward a modern theory of adaptive networks: expectation and prediction.Psychological Review, 88, 135–171.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thorndike, E. L. (1988). Animal Intelligence: an experimental study of the associative processes in animals.Psychological Review, Monograph Supplements, 2(8), 1–109.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thorndike, E. L. (1905).Elements of Psychology. A. G. Seiler, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thorndike, E. L. (1931).Human Learning. London, Century

    Google Scholar 

  • Thorndike, E. L. (1949).Selected Writings from a Connectionist's Psychology. Greenwood Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thorndike, E. L. and Lorge, I. (1944).The Teacher's Wordbook of 30,000 Words. Teacher's College, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. (1932).Purposive Behaviour in Animals and Men. Century, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. (1948). Cognitive maps in rats and men.Psychological Review, 55, 189–208.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turkle, S. (1988). Artificial Intelligence and Psychoanalysis: A new alliance.Deadelus, Winter, 241–268.

  • Walker, S. F. (1984).Learning Theory and Behaviour Modification. Methuen, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walker, S. F. (1985).Animal Thought. Routledge & Kegan Paul, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walker, S. F. (1987).Animal Learning. Routledge & Kegan Paul, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. B. (1913). Psychology as the behaviourist views it.Psychological Review, 20, 158–177.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. B. (1924).Psychology from the Standpoint of a Behaviourist. Lippincott, Philadelphia.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. B. (1925).Behaviourism. Kegan Paul, Trench & Trubner, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Widrow, G. and Hoff, M. E. (1960). Adaptive switching circuits.Institute of Radio Engineers, Western Electronic Show and Convention, Convention Record, Part 4, 393–395.

  • Wolpe, J. (1952). Experimental neurosis as learned behaviour.British Journal of Psychology, 43, 234–268.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolpe, J. (1958).Psychotherapy by Reciprocal Inhibition. Stanford University Press.

  • Wolpe, J. (1978). Cognition and causation in human behaviour and its therapy.American Psychologist, 33, 437–446.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolpe, J. (1981). Behavior therapy versus psychoanalysis: therapeutic and social implications.American Psychologist, 159–164.

  • Zeki, S. and Shipp, S. (1988). The functional logic of cortical connections.Nature, 335, 311–317.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zipser, D. (1986). Biologically plausible models of place recognition and goal location. In J. L. McClelland and D. E. Rumelhart (eds),Parallel Distributed Processing. Volume 2. Psychological and Biological Models. MIT Press, London, 432–470.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Walker, S.F. A brief history of connectionism and its psychological implications. AI & Soc 4, 17–38 (1990). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01889762

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01889762

Keywords

Navigation