iBet uBet web content aggregator. Adding the entire web to your favor.
iBet uBet web content aggregator. Adding the entire web to your favor.



Link to original content: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8313022
Personality and impulsivity as predictors of tobacco use among emerging adults: A latent class analysis - PMC Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2021 Sep 1.
Published in final edited form as: Pers Individ Dif. 2020 May 12;163:110076. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2020.110076

Personality and impulsivity as predictors of tobacco use among emerging adults: A latent class analysis

Jenny E Ozga-Hess a, Katelyn F Romm a, Nicholas J Felicione a, Geri Dino b,c, Melissa D Blank a, Nicholas A Turiano a,*
PMCID: PMC8313022  NIHMSID: NIHMS1648361  PMID: 34321706

Abstract

The tobacco industry markets their products toward emerging adults (18–29), with the goal of increasing use among this age group. To inform prevention efforts, researchers are investigating how specific demographic and psychological traits may predict tobacco initiation and continuation. Participants were 578 incoming university freshmen from the Appalachian region. Participants provided information on demographics, personality traits, impulsivity characteristics, lifetime use of cigarettes and electronic cigarettes (ECIGs), and current use of cigarettes, ECIGs, small cigars/cigarillos, large cigars, smokeless tobacco, and waterpipe. Latent class analysis identified tobacco-use classes and regressions identified psychological predictors of class membership. Participants were Nonusers, Experimenters, and Polytobacco Users. Lower agreeableness and conscientiousness as well as higher extraversion and neuroticism were associated with being Experimenters or Polytobacco Users. Lower impulsivity was associated with being Nonusers. Distinct types of emerging adults belong to each tobacco use class, suggesting that individual differences be incorporated in prevention efforts.

Keywords: Polytobacco, Emerging adult, College student, Prevention, Alternative tobacco product

1. Introduction

Many adult users report initiating use of a tobacco product before the age of 18 (CDC, 2014; Lipari & Van Horn, 2017; Sharapova et al., 2018). However, the transition from youth (ages 13–17) to emerging adulthood (ages 18–29; Arnett, 2014) is a critical developmental period whereby individuals experience many lifestyle changes and establish lifelong health-related behaviors (Arnett, Zukauskiene, & Sugimura, 2014). Emerging adulthood is characterized by changes in residence, relationships, financial status, and education (Arnett, 2000) and emerging adults gain new roles, responsibilities, and freedoms that are associated with increased engagement in a range of health-risk behaviors, including tobacco use (Lipari & Van Horn, 2017; SAMHSA, 2017; Wang et al., 2018). The tobacco industry has also targeted emerging adults specifically (Ling & Glantz, 2002) and given this historical targeting along with the legal purchase of tobacco products at 18 years of age in most states, it follows that use of tobacco products increases during the transition from youth to emerging adulthood (Lipari & Van Horn, 2017; SAMHSA, 2017; Wang et al., 2018).

Of concern is that with the rising popularity of alternative tobacco products such as electronic cigarettes (ECIGs), cigars, and hookah (Cullen et al., 2018; Evans-Polce, Lanza, & Maggs, 2016; Harrell, Naqvu, Plunk, Ji, & Martins, 2017), as well as openness to using such products among emerging adults (Mays et al., 2016), experimentation may lead to continued use and transitions between product types (Best, Haseen, Currie, Ozakinci, Mackintosh, & Stead, et al., 2017; Chaffee, Watkins, & Glantz, 2018; Cooke et al., 2016; Mays et al., 2016). Additional concerns arise when considering polytobacco use (i.e., use of two or more products concurrently), which is prevalent among emerging adult tobacco users (30.0–66.4%; Lisha, Thrul, & Ling, 2019; West et al., 2019), and may lead to more detrimental effects on the developing brain (Yuan, Cross, Loughlin, & Leslie, 2015) and later cessation attempts (Messer et al., 2015). Thus, prevention efforts need to be developed to reduce tobacco initiation among this age group. Although identifying individual risk factors are critical for these efforts (Dierker, Avenevoli, Goldberg, & Glantz, 2004), little is known about those factors outside of demographic (e.g., gender, race; Erickson, Lenk, & Forster, 2014; Evans-Polce et al., 2016; Harrell et al., 2017; Lisha et al., 2019) and tobacco-related characteristics (e.g., dependence level, access; Harrell et al., 2017; Lisha et al., 2019; Yu, Sacco, Choi, & Wintemberg, 2018).

Two potentially relevant factors may be personality and impulsivity given that these characteristics predict a multitude of health behaviors and risk for mortality (Bogg & Roberts, 2004; Hampson & Friedman, 2008; Turiano, Chapman, Gruenewald, & Mroczek, 2015). The Big Five model of personality includes the following characteristics—conscientiousness (e.g., goal-directed and in control), agreeableness (e.g., not hostile), neuroticism (e.g., emotional instability), extraversion (e.g., outgoing and sociable), and openness to experience (e.g., creative; McCrae & Costa Jr., 1997). Adolescent and adult cigarette smokers are significantly less conscientious, less agreeable, more neurotic, more extraverted, and more open to experience compared to non-smokers (Harakeh, Scholte, de Vries, & Engels, 2006; Malouff, Thorsteinsson, & Schutte, 2006; Terracciano & Costa Jr., 2004; Turiano, Whiteman, Hampson, Roberts, & Mroczek, 2012; Zvolensky, Taha, Bono, & Goodwin, 2015). Fewer studies have investigated the associations between personality and alternative product use, although existing work is in line with that reported for cigarette smokers. Specifically, users of smokeless tobacco are more extraverted and neurotic (Foreyt, Jackson, Squires, Hartung, Murray, & Gotto Jr., 1993), whereas cigar users are less conscientious (Brikmanis, Petersen, & Doran, 2017), than are nonusers of these products.

Impulsivity is defined as a tendency to act out in response to impulses and overlaps with some aspects of neuroticism and conscientiousness (Hofmann, Friese, & Strack, 2009; Um, Hershberger, Whitt, & Cyders, 2018). Impulsivity is a multidimensional construct that can be broken down into specific domains: lack of premeditation (i.e., acting without thinking), lack of perseverance (i.e., inability to remain focused), positive urgency (i.e., acting rashly in response to positive affective state), negative urgency (i.e., acting rashly in response to negative affective state), and sensation seeking (i.e., seeking out novel/thrilling experiences; Cyders, Smith, Spillane, Fischer, & Annus, 2007). Individuals that are more impulsive tend to engage in behaviors that provide short-term rewards at the expense of long-term benefits, including use of tobacco products. Specifically, lack of premeditation (Brikmanis et al., 2017), greater positive and negative urgencies (Doran & Trim, 2015; Leventhal et al., 2016), and greater sensation seeking (Brikmanis et al., 2017; Doran & Trim, 2015) are associated with cigarette, cigar, and ECIG use among emerging adults and adolescents. Polytobacco users may also be more impulsive than single product users (Leventhal et al., 2016).

Associations between personality/impulsivity characteristics and tobacco use are studied primarily using variable-centered approaches, in which relations are quantified using separate models. Person-centered approaches have the advantage of assessing how tobacco use behaviors interact and may better reflect the complexity of such behavior. One type of person-centered approach is Latent Class Analysis (LCA), which models latent subgroups within a population by clustering participants into classes based on their responses to specific questions. With the identification of tobacco use classes, various individual difference factors can be used to predict group membership. Understanding how these factors predict classes of tobacco use may aid in identifying those that are most likely to engage in the most problematic profiles of tobacco use (e.g., use of multiple products) and provide insight into which factors to potentially target during prevention efforts. Thus, the present secondary data analysis was designed to 1) use exploratory LCA to identify classes of emerging adult tobacco users and 2) to determine whether demographics, personality, and/or impulsivity characteristics predict class membership identified through exploratory LCA.

2. Materials and methods

2.1. Participants and procedures

Participants were 578 incoming freshmen enrolled at a large university in a mid-Atlantic state (Mage=18.13 years, SD=0.94; range=18–28 years). Most participants identified as female (69.62%), whereas 30.38% identified as male. For race, 89.43% identified as White, 2.94% identified as Black or African American, 1.73% identified as Asian/Native Hawaiian or Pacific Islander, 0.69% identified as American Indian or Alaskan Native, 3.63% identified as more than one race, and 1.56% identified as a race not listed. Participants were from West Virginia (53.97%), Pennsylvania (14.26%), Maryland (6.06%), Ohio (5.88%), Virginia (5.02%), New Jersey (3.81%), or another state (11.00%).

Participants were recruited via email from a list of incoming freshmen provided by the university's Office of Enrollment Management. An email was sent to all incoming freshmen (approximately 5000 students) in July 2016 asking students to consent to the study via SurveyMonkey. The survey was closed after 578 responses were received. Surveys were completed online during the summer before participants arrived on campus for their first semester of college. Participants received $20 for completing the survey. All outlined procedures were approved by the university's Institutional Review Board.

2.2. Measures

Data were collected as part of a larger project aimed at understanding demographic and psychological factors that predict prospective patterns of various types of substance use among college students over their freshman year.

2.2.1. Demographic variables

Participants were asked to indicate whether they identified as male or female. Subjective familial socioeconomic status (SES) was assessed using the MacArthur Scale of Subjective Social Status (Goodman et al., 2001), which includes an image of a 10-rung ladder with the instructions: “Imagine that this ladder shows how your society is set up. Now think about your family. Please tell us where you think your family would be on this ladder.” The top rung (coded as a 10) was labeled “the people who are best off – they have the most money, the highest amount of schooling, and the jobs that bring the most respect” and the bottom rung (coded as a 1) was labeled “the people who are the worst off – they have the least money, little or no education, no jobs or jobs that no one wants or respects.”

2.2.2. Big Five personality traits

Personality was assessed using the Big Five Inventory (BFI-2; Soto & John, 2017). The BFI-2 is a 60-item self-report with subscales measuring openness to experience (e.g., “I am a person who is curious about many different things.”; 12 items; α=0.80), conscientiousness (e.g., “I am a person who is dependable, steady.”; 12 items; α=0.85), extraversion (e.g., “I am a person who is outgoing, sociable.”; 12 items; α=0.86), agreeableness (e.g., “I am a person who is compassionate, has a soft heart.”; 12 items; α=0.81), and neuroticism (e.g., “I am a person who worries a lot.”; 12 items; α=0.89). Participants indicated how much they agreed with each item on a five-point Likert scale (1=Strongly disagree, 5=Strongly agree). Mean scores were computed by averaging scores for each of the items that comprise each subscale, with higher scores indicating higher levels of each trait.

2.2.3. Impulsivity

The UPPS-P Impulsivity Scale was used to assess five distinct features of impulsive behavior (Cyders et al., 2007; Lynam, Smith, Whiteside, & Cyders, 2006): negative urgency (e.g., “Sometimes I do impulsive things that I later regret.”; 12 items, α=0.84), positive urgency (e.g., “When I am very happy, I feel that it is okay to give in to cravings or overindulge.”; 14 items; α=0.94), lack of perseverance (e.g., “Sometimes there are so many little things to be done that I just ignore them all.”; 10 items; α=0.60), lack of premeditation (e.g., “Before making up my mind, I consider all the advantages and disadvantages.”; reverse coded; 11 items; α=0.84), and sensation seeking (e.g., “I would enjoy fast driving.”; 12 items; α=0.85). Each item was rated on a 4-point Likert scale (1=Disagree strongly, 4=Agree strongly). Mean scores were computed by averaging scores for each of the items that comprise each subscale, with higher scores indicating higher levels of each feature of impulsive behavior.

2.2.4. Tobacco use

Tobacco use was quantified by lifetime use of cigarettes or ECIGs and current use of cigarettes, ECIGs, large cigars, small cigars/cigarillos, smokeless tobacco, or waterpipe. Lifetime use was not assessed for large cigars, small cigars/cigarillos, smokeless tobacco, or waterpipe due to an error at baseline in the SurveyMonkey questionnaire. Given that the primary aim of data collection was to evaluate prospective patterns of substance use, participants were not re-contacted to obtain lifetime history of other tobacco products. Participants were asked, “Have you ever tried a cigarette (even if only one puff) in your lifetime?” (0=No, 1=Yes) and “Have you ever used electronic cigarettes (e.g. e-cigarettes)?” (0=No, 1=Yes). The latter question was followed by images of examples of ECIGs that included first- and second-generation devices. If participants indicated that they had tried a cigarette or an ECIG in their lifetime, they were asked, “Do you currently smoke cigarettes?” or “Are you currently using e-cigarettes?”, respectively (0=No, 1=Yes). Participants were also asked, “Have you used any of the following tobacco products in the past 30 days?”, which was followed by a list of products including large cigars, small cigars/cigarillos, smokeless tobacco (e.g., snuff/dip/chew/snus), and waterpipe (e.g., hookah, shisha). Any use in the past 30 days was classified as current use of that product (0=No, 1=Yes).

2.3. Analytic plan

Analyses were conducted using Mplus 8.1 (Muthén & Muthén, 1998–2002). Exploratory LCAs determined classes of tobacco use among emerging adults. LCA is a person-centered approach that identifies classes of individuals that exhibit similar patterns of scores across categorical indicators and estimates the probability of each participant belonging to each class (Nylund, 2007). The number of classes was determined empirically based on fit indexes, such as the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC; Akaike, 1974) and the Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC; Nylund, Asparouhov, & Muthén, 2007), for which lower scores represent better fit. The Vuong Lo-Mendal Rubin LRT test, which evaluates whether a model with k profiles provides a significant improvement in fit over a model with k-1 profiles, was used to determine the class solution that best fit the data (Lo, Mendell, & Rubin, 2001; Vuong, 1989). Models with high entropy were also given preference. Starting with a one-profile solution, models were estimated with increasingly more profiles until there was no further model improvement (i.e., fit indexes show no substantive change or additional profiles are small, conceptually unclear, or there are slight variations on already identified profiles; Nylund, 2007).

Once latent classes were identified, separate multinomial logistic regressions were examined to determine whether specific personality and impulsivity characteristics predicted class membership after accounting for demographics. Multinomial logistic regression compares multiple groups through a combination of binary logistic regressions in one model. Data met the assumptions for use of multinomial logistic regression, including the use of categorical dependent variables and continuous independent variables, independence of observations, and linear associations among predictors and tobacco variables at the bivariate level (see Table 1). Multicollinearity was not a concern, as all variance inflation factor (VIF) values were below 5 (Hair, Ringle, & Sarstedt, 2011; see Table 1). Models were built in a stepwise fashion. To ensure that findings from our final model were not due to suppression, we first examined a model (Model 1) that included demographics only (i.e., gender, SES). Model 2 added each of the Big Five personality traits. Model 3 included demographics and impulsivity characteristics. The final model (Model 4) included demographic, personality, and impulsivity characteristics. Predictors of class membership were considered statistically significant when p < .05.

Table 1.

Correlations, descriptive statistics, and VIFs for key study variables.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20
1. Gender (male)
2. SES 0.01
3. Extraversion −0.02 0.19
4. Agreeableness −0.14 0.02 0.20
5. Conscientiousness −0.13 0.02 0.20 0.41
6. Neuroticism −0.29 −0.13 −0.37 −0.36 −0.28
7. Openness −0.07 −0.05 0.19 0.22 0.11 −0.04
8. Negative Urgency −0.05 −0.04 −0.19 −0.44 −0.48 0.56 −0.20
9. Positive Urgency 0.16 0.02 −0.03 −0.37 −0.47 0.18 −0.21 0.67
10. Lack of Premed. 0.05 −0.01 0.11 −0.24 −0.48 0.09 −0.16 0.35 0.41
11. Lack of Persev. 0.01 −0.10 −0.32 −0.29 −0.70 0.32 −0.21 0.45 0.39 0.50
12. Sensation Seeking 0.18 0.07 0.32 0.01 −0.03 −0.26 0.11 0.03 0.23 0.12 −0.15
13. Life. Cigarette 0.01 −0.08 0.02 −0.08 −0.12 0.14 0.07 0.13 0.11 0.20 0.09 0.01
14. Life. ECIG 0.04 −0.04 −0.02 −0.10 −0.17 0.09 0.01 0.17 0.10 0.13 0.10 0.14 0.34
15. Cur. Cigarette 0.03 0.02 0.09 −0.10 −0.09 0.05 0.08 0.07 0.07 0.13 0.06 0.06 0.39 0.18
16. Cur. ECIG 0.10 −0.01 0.03 −0.15 −0.12 0.03 0.05 0.12 0.14 0.09 0.03 0.10 0.22 0.33 0.27
17. Cur. SLT 0.25 −0.02 0.02 −0.11 −0.04 −0.03 −0.04 −0.03 0.02 0.01 −0.04 0.03 0.13 0.23 0.17 0.28
18. Cur. Large Cigar 0.34 0.02 0.05 −0.17 −0.10 −0.02 −0.01 0.01 0.08 0.09 −0.02 0.13 0.10 0.27 0.11 0.29 0.36
19. Cur. Small Cigar 0.13 −0.05 0.02 −0.16 −0.12 −0.01 −0.06 0.08 0.09 0.10 0.06 0.10 0.17 0.36 0.09 0.24 0.32 0.44
20. Cur. Waterpipe 0.03 −0.06 0.02 −0.17 −0.14 0.08 −0.06 0.17 0.16 0.10 0.13 0.08 0.10 0.22 0.14 0.25 0.19 0.18 0.35
M / % Users 30.2% 6.00 3.46 3.72 3.61 2.91 3.74 2.18 1.74 1.96 1.87 2.88 10.6% 28.1% 3.1% 4.0% 4.0% 7.1% 15.1% 7.5%
SD 1.66 0.72 0.61 0.66 0.81 0.61 0.59 0.60 0.46 0.48 0.59
VIF 1.26 1.06 1.51 1.49 2.46 2.14 1.19 2.99 2.34 1.67 2.56 1.33

Note. Bolded values denote statistical significance (p < .05). Some variable names have been shortened. Premed.=Premeditation; Persever.=Perseverance; Life.=Lifetime; Cur.=Current; SLT=smokeless tobacco use.

3. Results

3.1. Latent class descriptions

A LCA utilizing eight dichotomous indicators of nicotine/tobacco use (i.e., lifetime cigarette, lifetime ECIG, current cigarette, current ECIG, current smokeless tobacco, current small cigar/cigarillo, current large cigar, current waterpipe) indicated that a three-profile solution fit the data well (see Table 2). The three-profile solution was most valid conceptually, had high entropy, and included classes of sufficient sample size.

Table 2.

Fit indices for latent class analyses.

Number of
profiles
AIC BIC LRT Test (p
value)
Entropy Smallest profile
(% of sample)
2 2378.734 2452.876 < .001 0.805 21.28%
3 2345.815 2376.669 .173 0.822 9.86%
4 2332.760 2484.760 .008 0.845 2.42%
5 2333.403 2525.300 .054 0.882 2.42%

Fig. 1 displays variable probabilities for each latent class. Class 1 represents Experimenters; they showed the highest levels of lifetime cigarette use and high levels of lifetime ECIG use, but engaged in little current use of any product (9.9% of participants). Class 2 was named Nonusers; individuals in this group engaged in the lowest levels of lifetime and current use of each product (78.1% of participants). Finally, Class 3 was named Polytobacco Users; they reported relatively high levels of lifetime ECIG use as well as of current use of smokeless tobacco, large cigars, small cigars/cigarillos, and waterpipe (12.1% of participants). For each of these latent classes, means and standard deviations for measured personality and impulsivity variables are presented in Table 3.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1.

Conditional probabilities of endorsing the tobacco use items for the three latent classes. Class 1 was labeled Experimenters, Class 2 was labeled Nonusers, and Class 3 was labeled Polytobacco Users.

Table 3.

Descriptive statistics for key study variables by smoking class.

Nonusers
Polytobacco Users
Experimenters
M SD M SD M SD
Extraversion 3.45 0.75 3.54 0.53 3.43 0.73
Agreeableness 3.77 0.60 3.42 0.63 3.66 0.61
Conscientiousness 3.66 0.66 3.36 0.63 3.47 0.63
Neuroticism 2.88 0.81 2.89 0.78 3.16 0.80
Openness 3.74 0.61 3.66 0.65 3.83 0.60
Negative Urgency 2.13 0.59 2.29 0.57 2.39 0.61
Positive Urgency 1.70 0.58 1.95 0.67 1.80 0.63
Lack of Premeditation 1.91 0.45 2.09 0.47 2.15 0.45
Lack of Perseverance 1.84 0.48 1.95 0.46 1.97 0.47
Sensation Seeking 2.85 0.57 3.04 0.59 2.88 0.73

Note. Personality variables ranged from 1 (Strongly Disagree) to 5 (Strongly Agree); impulsivity variables ranged from 1 (Disagree Strongly) to 4 (Agree Strongly).

3.2. Associations with latent classes

Statistical outcomes for differences in class membership based on demographics, personality, and impulsivity are shown in Table 4.

Table 4.

Multinomial logistic regressions predicting smoking classes from demographic characteristics, personality traits, and impulsivity variables with Nonusers and Polytobacco Users as referent groups.

Exp (vs. Poly)
Non (vs. Poly)
Poly (vs. Exp)
Non (vs. Exp)
Exp (vs. Non)
Poly (vs. Non)
B (SE B) OR B (SE B) OR B (SE B) OR B (SE B) OR B (SE B) OR B (SE B) OR
Model 1
Gender 1.52 (0.41) 4.55 1.26 (0.27) 3.52 −1.44 (0.40) 0.24 −0.19 (0.34) 0.83 0.26 (0.35) 1.30 −1.26 (0.27) 0.28
SES −0.15 (0.11) 0.86 0.01 (0.08) 1.01 0.14 (0.11) 1.15 0.15 (0.09) 1.16 −0.16 (0.09) 0.85 −0.01 (0.08) 0.99
Model 2
Gender 1.28 (0.43) 3.59 1.15 (0.29) 3.15 −1.28 (0.43) 0.28 −0.13 (0.36) 0.88 0.13 (0.36) 1.14 −1.15 (0.29) 0.32
SES −0.11 (0.11) 0.90 0.05 (0.08) 1.05 0.11 (0.11) 1.12 0.16 (0.09) 1.17 −0.16 (0.09) 0.86 −0.05 (0.08) 0.95
Extraversion −0.17 (0.26) 0.85 −0.43 (0.18) 0.65 0.17 (0.26) 1.18 −0.27 (0.22) 0.77 0.27 (0.22) 1.31 0.43 (0.18) 1.54
Agreeableness 0.60 (0.33) 1.81 0.68 (0.23) 1.97 −0.60 (0.33) 0.55 0.08 (0.28) 1.09 −0.08 (0.28) 0.92 −0.68 (0.23) 0.51
Conscientiousness 0.01 (0.30) 1.01 0.42 (0.22) 1.52 −0.01 (0.30) 0.99 0.41 (0.24) 1.51 −0.41 (0.24) 0.66 −0.42 (0.22) 0.66
Neuroticism 0.30 (0.27) 1.35 −0.07 (0.20) 0.93 −0.30 (0.27) 0.74 −0.37 (0.20) 0.69 0.37 (0.20) 1.44 0.07 (0.20) 1.07
Openness 0.30 (0.30) 1.34 0.11 (0.21) 1.11 −0.30 (0.30) 0.74 −0.19 (0.25) 0.83 0.19 (0.25) 1.21 −0.11 (0.21) 0.90
Model 3
Gender 1.30 (0.43) 3.66 1.16 (0.27) 3.19 −1.30 (0.43) 0.27 −0.14 (0.37) 0.87 0.14 (0.37) 1.15 −1.16 (0.27) 0.31
SES −0.14 (0.11) 0.87 0.02 (0.08) 1.02 0.14 (0.11) 1.15 0.16 (0.09) 1.17 −0.16 (0.09) 0.85 −0.02 (0.08) 0.98
Negative Urgency 0.41 (0.42) 1.51 −0.32 (0.33) 0.73 −0.41 (0.42) 0.66 −0.73 (0.32) 0.48 0.73 (0.32) 2.08 0.32 (0.33) 1.38
Positive Urgency −0.51 (0.42) 0.60 −0.10 (0.32) 0.90 0.51 (0.42) 1.66 0.41 (0.34) 1.50 −0.41 (0.34) 0.67 0.10 (0.32) 1.11
Lack of Premeditation 0.61 (0.50) 1.84 −0.62 (0.37) 0.54 −0.61 (0.50) 0.54 −1.23 (0.40) 0.29 1.23 (0.40) 3.42 0.62 (0.37) 1.86
Lack of Perseverance −0.35 (0.46) 0.71 −0.07 (0.36) 0.93 0.35 (0.46) 1.41 0.27 (0.36) 1.31 −0.27 (0.36) 0.76 0.07 (0.36) 1.08
Sensation Seeking −0.28 (0.40) 0.76 −0.31 (0.27) 0.73 0.28 (0.40) 1.32 −0.04 (0.32) 0.96 0.04 (0.32) 1.04 0.31 (0.27) 1.37
Model 4
Gender 1.14 (0.45) 3.11 1.07 (0.30) 2.93 −1.14 (0.45) 0.32 −0.06 (0.39) 0.94 0.06 (0.39) 1.06 −1.07 (0.30) 0.34
SES −0.10 (0.11) 0.90 0.05 (0.08) 1.05 0.10 (0.11) 1.11 0.15 (0.09) 1.15 −0.15 (0.09) 0.86 −0.05 (0.08) 0.95
Extraversion −0.25 (0.30) 0.78 −0.29 (21) 0.65 0.25 (0.30) 1.28 −0.04 (0.25) 0.96 0.04 (0.25) 1.05 0.29 (0.21) 1.44
Agreeableness 0.68 (0.34) 1.98 0.67 (0.24) 1.95 −0.68 (0.34) 0.41 −0.02 (0.28) 0.98 0.02 (0.28) 1.02 −0.67 (0.24) 0.31
Conscientiousness 0.14 (0.45) 1.15 0.29 (0.34) 1.34 −0.14 (0.45) 0.87 0.15 (0.35) 1.54 −0.15 (0.35) 0.66 −0.29 (0.34) 0.45
Neuroticism 0.08 (0.34) 1.09 −0.10 (0.23) 0.90 −0.08 (0.34) 0.92 −0.19 (0.27) 0.53 0.19 (0.27) 1.41 0.10 (0.23) 1.11
Openness 0.38 (0.35) 1.47 0.09 (0.23) 1.09 −0.38 (0.35) 0.68 −0.30 (0.28) 0.75 0.30 (0.28) 1.34 −0.09 (0.23) 0.92
Negative Urgency 0.60 (0.53) 1.83 0.01 (0.39) 1.01 −0.60 (0.53) 0.55 −0.60 (0.41) 0.55 0.60 (0.41) 1.82 −0.01 (0.39) 0.99
Positive Urgency −0.35 (0.41) 0.71 −0.01 (0.33) 1.01 0.35 (0.41) 1.42 0.35 (0.34) 1.41 −0.35 (0.34) 0.71 0.01 (0.33) 1.01
Lack of Premeditation 0.83 (0.53) 1.30 −0.37 (0.39) 0.69 −0.83 (0.53) 0.44 −1.21 (0.42) 0.30 1.21 (0.42) 3.34 0.37 (0.39) 1.45
Lack of Perseverance −0.29 (0.59) 0.75 0.07 (0.48) 1.07 0.29 (0.59) 1.34 0.36 (0.43) 1.44 −0.36 (0.43) 0.70 −0.07 (0.48) 0.93
Sensation Seeking −0.28 (0.45) 0.76 −0.30 (0.31) 0.74 0.28 (0.45) 1.32 −0.02 (0.34) 0.98 0.02 (0.34) 1.02 0.30 (0.31) 1.35

Note. Bolded values denote statistical significance (p < .05). Some variable names have been shortened; Poly=Polytobacco Users; Non=Nonusers; Exp=Experimenters.

3.2.1. Demographic variables

Being female was associated with increased odds of being in the Experimenters and the Nonusers classes compared to the Polytobacco Users class. Higher SES was associated with increased odds of being in the Nonusers compared to the Experimenters class.

3.2.2. Big Five personality traits

After accounting for demographic variables, individuals with higher levels of agreeableness were at increased odds of being in the Nonusers and the Experimenters classes compared to the Polytobacco Users class, whereas individuals with higher levels of conscientiousness were at increased odds for belonging to the Nonusers class compared to both the Experimenters and Polytobacco Users classes. Additionally, individuals higher in extraversion were at increased odds of being in the Polytobacco Users than the Nonusers class and individuals higher in neuroticism were at increased odds for belonging to the Experimenters compared to the Nonusers class.

3.2.3. Impulsivity

Controlling for demographics, individuals with a greater lack of premeditation were at increased odds for belonging to the Experimenters and Polytobacco Users classes compared to the Nonusers class. Additionally, individuals with greater negative urgency were at increased odds for belonging to the Experimenters class compared to the Nonusers class.

4. Discussion

This secondary data analysis was conducted to identify classes of tobacco users among emerging adults and to subsequently assess whether personality or impulsivity characteristics are associated with class membership. Consistent with prior work (Cooke et al., 2016; Erickson et al., 2014), a majority of the emerging adults in the current sample reported limited lifetime or current product use, and LCA combined these participants into the largest class (78.1% Nonusers). There was sufficient variability in product use patterns for the remaining 21.9% of the sample, leading to the identification of two discrete classes of users. The largest of these classes was Polytobacco Users (12.1%), followed by Experimenters (9.9%). High probabilities of lifetime ECIG use were evident in both classes, suggesting that experimentation with ECIGs is common among emerging adults (Best et al., 2017; Cooke et al., 2016; Mays et al., 2016). Notable is that cigarette use is declining among emerging adults accompanied by an increase in use of alternative products (Cullen et al., 2018; Evans-Polce et al., 2016). Indeed, lifetime cigarette use was more prevalent for Experimenters than Polytobacco Users in the current study, though current use of cigarettes was uncommon for both classes. Instead, those in the Polytobacco Users class engaged in higher rates of alternative product use, including use of smokeless tobacco, waterpipe, large cigars, and small cigars/cigarillos.

The three classes identified by LCA were also diverse in regard to demographic characteristics. Not surprisingly, males were at increased odds of being Polytobacco Users as compared to Nonusers and Experimenters. These findings replicate a large body of research demonstrating that males are more likely to use smokeless tobacco (Agaku & Alpert, 2016; Erickson et al., 2014) and cigars (Agaku & Alpert, 2016; Phillips et al., 2017), and also to engage in polytobacco use (Phillips et al., 2017; Soneji, Sargent, & Tanski, 2016), relative to their female counterparts. Participants of higher SES were also less likely to be Experimenters and more likely to be Nonusers, though other differences in SES between classes were not observed. Among adolescent samples, use of traditional tobacco products (cigarettes, smokeless tobacco) is associated reliably with lower SES (Wellman et al., 2016; Wellman et al., 2018). Findings for other products (little cigars, cigarillos, ECIGs) are mixed, however (Krishnan-Sarin et al., 2019; Simon et al., 2017). In contrast to this previous work, our measure of SES was subjective (Goodman et al., 2001). A myriad of assessments exist for SES (Shavers, 2007), and more objective measures, such as parental income, may have revealed differences between non- and current users. Still, emerging adults may not be the best reporters of parental income (Svedberg, Nygren, Staland-Nyman, & Nyholm, 2016).

After controlling for demographic factors, personality and impulsivity characteristics were associated significantly with class membership. With regard to personality, agreeableness, conscientiousness, neuroticism, and extraversion varied across classes, indicating that such individual-difference factors play a role in initiation and continuation of nicotine/tobacco product use (Foreyt et al., 1993). Consistent with studies using variable-centered analytic approaches (Harakeh et al., 2006; Malouff et al., 2006; Terracciano & Costa Jr., 2004), individuals scoring lower on agreeableness were more likely to be in classes associated with current product use (Polytobacco Users) and less likely to be Nonusers or Experimenters. There are significant social pressures against experimenting with and continuing to use nicotine/tobacco products (Castaldelli-Maia, Ventriglio, & Bhugra, 2016), and individuals who score lower on agreeableness tend to be rebellious, with lower needs for social approval than those scoring higher on this trait (Terracciano & Costa Jr., 2004).

Arguably the most consistent finding in the personality/tobacco literature (Harakeh et al., 2006; Malouff et al., 2006; Zvolensky et al., 2015), individuals scoring higher on conscientiousness were more likely to be Nonusers than Experimenters or Polytobacco Users in the current study. Those that are more conscientious tend to take fewer health risks, engage in more beneficial health-related behaviors (e.g., healthy diet, regular exercise), and consequently live longer than their counterparts (Bogg & Roberts, 2004; Hampson & Friedman, 2008). Further, two facets of impulsivity were associated with tobacco class membership, with Nonusers scoring lower than Experimenters and Polytobacco Users on measures of negative urgency and lack of premeditation. Given that negative urgency and premeditation overlap with aspects of conscientiousness (Hofmann et al., 2009; Um et al., 2018), these results highlight the critical role of self-control in abstinence from nicotine/tobacco use.

Higher levels of neuroticism and extraversion are consistently associated with cigarette smoking and smokeless tobacco use among adolescent, adult, and college student samples (Foreyt et al., 1993; Harakeh et al., 2006; Malouff et al., 2006; Zvolensky et al., 2015). In this vein, Polytobacco Users and Experimenters scored higher on measures of extraversion and neuroticism, respectively, compared to Nonusers in the current study. It is possible that those scoring higher on these traits experiment with or use nicotine/tobacco for stimulation or to reduce tension/anxiety (Eysenck, 2012; Gonzalez, Zvolensky, Vujanovic, Leyro, & Marshall, 2008). Of course, reasons for experimentation or use cannot be determined from the present data and future work will be needed to evaluate this possibility. Notably, openness to experience did not predict latent class membership in the present study. The relation between openness and nicotine/tobacco use may be unreliable (Terracciano & Costa Jr., 2004; Zvolensky et al., 2015), and thus requires further systematic study, especially with the availability of new tobacco products such as ECIGs.

Together, findings regarding personality and impulsivity highlight that distinct types of emerging adults belong to each tobacco use class. Given that individual-difference factors play an important role in the effectiveness of prevention and intervention efforts (Dierker et al., 2004), different classes of individuals may require different strategies. Although it was once thought that personality and impulsivity characteristics are stable across the lifespan (McCrae & Costa Jr., 1997; Odum, 2011), recent interventions were developed to change these directly in an effort to improve various health-related behaviors (e.g., Hudson & Fraley, 2015; Roberts et al., 2017; Stein et al., 2016). For instance, episodic future thinking asks individuals to imagine future events to increase the value of delayed consequences and reduce impulsivity (Atance & O'Neill, 2001). Such an intervention is successful in reducing cigarette consumption, at least in the short term (Stein et al., 2016). For personality, self-directed goal setting may be adequate for those that want to change their personality traits, while others may require behavioral or cognitive intervention (Hudson & Fraley, 2015; Roberts et al., 2017). Indeed, the sociogenomic model of personality suggests that small modifications in behavioral, attitudinal, or emotional changes can eventually become habitual and lead to lasting change (Roberts & Jackson, 2008). Assuming that changes in personality/impulsivity translate to improvements in health-related behaviors, grouping individuals together into classes of tobacco use patterns may hold promise for tailoring interventions to tobacco use prevention. Thus, it may be important for colleges and universities to have different types of prevention and intervention programs in place to tailor treatment to specific types of emerging adults. There may also be utility in pre-testing college students and using this information to appropriately target tobacco prevention and intervention programs.

Although the current study provides valuable insight into the complexity of tobacco use behaviors among a sample of emerging adults, results must be interpreted in light of some important considerations. First, the sample included participants that were from a single large, public university and were predominantly white/Caucasian. However, the majority of participants came from areas of West Virginia, Ohio, and Pennsylvania that encompass the greater Appalachian region. This region is notable due to disproportionately high rates of cigarette and smokeless tobacco use (American Lung Association, 2015) as well as the substantial increases in ECIG, hookah, and polytobacco use since 2013 (WV DHHR, 2015). With the rate of tobacco industry marketing expenditures being nearly 12 times higher than the national rate (Campaign for Tobacco Free Kids, 2019), combined with annual funding for tobacco control efforts at only ~17.8% of the CDC-recommended amount (CDC, 2019), future studies need to further explore such at-risk populations to understand factors that predict initiation and continuation of tobacco use.1 Moreover, the current study focused on the transition into college and thus, participants fell primarily on the lower end of the age range classified as emerging adults. Future research should examine preditors of tobacco use among a wider age range of university-attending and non-university-attending emerging adults, given that tobacco use of those who attend college may differ from those who do not (Lenk et al., 2012).

Although these data were collected relatively recently (2016), tobacco use patterns change quickly (Delk et al., 2019) and the cross-sectional nature of the data do not allow for assessing changes in class membership over time. In addition, a restricted number of product types and levels of use were assessed in the current study. For product types, the item used to assess smokeless tobacco (snuff/dip/chew/snus) use was aggregate in nature; what type(s) of smokeless tobacco products were more popular (traditional (snuff/chew) or newer (snus)) cannot be discerned from the present data. With regard to frequency, lifetime use was evaluated only for cigarettes and ECIGs whereby current use was assessed for all product types. However, as mentioned previously, this secondary data analysis included all available tobacco use items. Additional questions about lifetime use of other alternative products may further divide the Experimenters class into groups of individuals that experiment with specific products. In future studies, it will be important to tease apart more nuanced differences between classes.

Another consideration is that questions regarding ECIG use referred to such devices as “electronic cigarettes” and included example images of first- and second-generation devices only. It is possible that participants responded “no” to these questions based on their familiarity with alternative language (e.g., “vape”; Hinds 3rd et al., 2016) and/or newer devices (e.g., mods, Juul; Barrington-Trimis et al., 2018), leading to underestimation of lifetime/current ECIG use among the current sample. Moreover, these data were collected right before individuals started college; it is possible that tobacco use increased when individuals were out of their homes and exposed to settings where tobacco use is more likely to occur (Lipperman-Kreda, Paschall, Robert, & Morrison, 2018). Finally, relatively small mean and standard deviation values for several personality and impulsivity traits suggest that the majority of participants scored toward the lower end of the scales (e.g., range of 1.70 to 1.95 for negative urgency on a scale from 1 to 4) and that there was little variability between participants. Thus, it will be important to replicate these findings with participants that produce a wider range of values and score at the higher end of these scales. However, the fact that many of these characteristics still significantly predicted latent class membership, despite their restricted range, indicates that they are likely strong indicators of tobacco use patterns.

The present results describe an important role for personality and impulsivity in nicotine/tobacco use behavior among emerging adults. Individuals that engage in use of multiple tobacco products are also at greater risk for use of other substances, such as marijuana and alcohol (Bernstein et al., 2015; Pulvers et al., 2018), and the associations between demographics, personality, or impulsivity and tobacco use observed here are consistent with those reported for other drugs (Chuang et al., 2017; Dash et al., 2019; Slade et al., 2016). Historically, focus has been toward preventing uptake of tobacco use among adolescents; however, given the relatively high prevalence of tobacco initiation during the transition from youth to emerging adulthood (Lipari & Van Horn, 2017; SAMHSA, 2017; Wang et al., 2018), prevention and intervention techniques need to be developed for emerging adults specifically. Because personality and impulsivity characteristics are amenable to change (Hudson & Fraley, 2015; Roberts et al., 2017), it is possible to tailor prevention to those that are most at risk for tobacco use.

Acknowledgments

Funding

This work was supported by the National Institute of General Medical Sciences (NIGMS U54GM104942-03) and the Centers for Disease Control (CDC U48 DP005004) to the WV Prevention Research Center.

Footnotes

1

We did not have data detailed enough to determine whether participants were from Appalachia in the current study, but a follow-up sensitivity analysis by participants' state of origin revealed no differences.

References

  1. Agaku IT, & Alpert HR (2016). Trends in annual sales and current use of cigarettes, cigars, roll-your-own tobacco, pipes, and smokeless tobacco among US adults, 2002–2012. Tobacco Control 25, 451–457. 10.1136/tobaccocontrol-2014-052125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Akaike H (1974). A new look at the statistical model identification. IEEE Transactions on Automatic Control 19, 716–723. 10.1109/TAC.1974.1100705. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  3. American Lung Association (2015). Cutting tobacco's rural roots: Tobacco use in rural communities. Chicago: American Lung Association; (Accessed September 23, 2019). [Google Scholar]
  4. Arnett JJ (2000). Emerging adulthood: A theory from the late teens through the twenties. American Psychologist, 55, 469–480. 10.1037/0003-066X.55.5.469. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Arnett JJ (2014). Presidential address: The emergence of emerging adulthood: A personal history. Emerging Adulthood, 2, 155–162. 10.1177/2167696814541096. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  6. Arnett JJ, Zukauskiene R, & Sugimura K (2014). The new life stage of emerging adulthood at ages 18–29 years: Implications for mental health. Lancet Psychiatry, 1, 569–576. 10.1016/S2215-0366(14)00080-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Atance CM, & O'Neill DK (2001). Episodic future thinking. Trends in Cognitive Science, 5, 533–539. 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01804-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Barrington-Trimis JL, Gibson LA, Halpern-Felsher B, Harrell MB, Kong G, Krishnan-Sarin S, … Weaver SR (2018). Type of e-cigarette device used among adolescents and young adults: Findings from a pooled analysis of eight studies of 2166 vapers. Nicotine and Tobacco Research, 20, 271–274. 10.1093/ntr/ntx069. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bernstein MH, McSheffrey SN, van den Berg JJ, Vela JE, Stein LA, Roberts MB, … Clarke JG (2015). The association between impulsivity and alcohol/drug use among prison inmates. Addictive Behaviors, 42, 140–143. 10.1016/j.addbeh.2014.11.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Best C, Haseen F, Currie D, Ozakinci G, MacKintosh AM, Stead M, … Haw S (2017). Relationship between trying an electronic cigarette and subsequent cigarette experimentation in Scottish adolescents: A cohort study. Tobacco Control. 10.1136/tobaccocontrol-2017-053691 pii. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bogg T, & Roberts BW (2004). Conscientiousness and health-related behaviors: A meta-analysis of the leading behavioral contributors to mortality. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 887–919. 10.1037/0033-2909.130.6.887. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Brikmanis K, Petersen A, & Doran N (2017). Do personality traits related to affect regulation predict other tobacco product use among young adult non-daily smokers? Addictive Behaviors, 75, 79–84. 10.1016/j.addbeh.2017.07.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Campaign for Tobacco Free Kids (2019). State-specific estimates of tobacco company marketing expenditures 1998 to 2017. (Accessed September 23, 2019).
  14. Castaldelli-Maia JM, Ventriglio A, & Bhugra D (2016). Tobacco smoking: From ‘glamour’ to ‘stigma’. A comprehensive review. Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 70, 24–33. 10.1111/pcn.12365. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) (2014). National Center for Chronic Disease Prevention and Health Promotion. The health consequences of smoking-50 years of progress: A report of the surgeon general. Office on Smoking and Health. [Google Scholar]
  16. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) (2019). Extinguishing the tobacco epidemic in West Virginia. (Accessed September 23, 2019).
  17. Chaffee BW, Watkins SL, & Glantz SA (2018). Electronic cigarette use and progression from experimentation to established smoking. Pediatrics, 141. 10.1542/peds.2017-3594 pii: e20173594. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Chuang CI, Sussman S, Stone MD, Pang RD, Chou CP, Leventhal AM, & Kirkpatrick MG (2017). Impulsivity and history of behavioral addictions is associated with drug use in adolescents. Addictive Behaviors, 74, 41–47. 10.1016/j.addbeh.2017.05.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Cooke ME, Nasim A, Cho SB, Kendler KS, Clark SL, & Dick DM (2016). Predicting tobacco use across the first year of college. American Journal of Health Behavior, 40, 484–495. 10.5993/AJHB.40.4.10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Cullen KA, Ambrose BK, Gentzke A, Apelberg BJ, Jamal A, & King BA (2018). Notes from the field: Use of electronic cigarettes and any tobacco product among middle and high school students — United States, 2011-2018. MMWR Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 68, 1276–1277. 10.15585/mmwr.mm6745a5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Cyders MA, Smith GT, Spillane NS, Fischer S, & Annus AM (2007). Integration of impulsivity and positive mood to predict risky behavior: Development and validation of a measure of positive urgency. Psychological Assessment, 19, 107–118. 10.1037/1040-3590.19.1.107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Dash GF, Slutske WS, Martin NG, Statham DJ, Agrawal A, & Lynskey MT (2019). Big Five personality traits and alcohol, nicotine, cannabis, and gambling disorder comorbidity. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 33, 420–429. 10.1037/adb0000468. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Delk J, Carey FR, Case KR, Creamer MR, Wilkinson AV, Perry CL, & Harrell MB (2019). Adolescent tobacco uptake and other substance use: A latent class analysis. American Journal of Health Behavior, 43, 3–14. 10.5993/AJHB.43.1.1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Dierker LS, Avenevoli S, Goldberg A, & Glantz M (2004). Defining subgroups of adolescents at risk for experimental and regular smoking. Preventative Science, 5, 169–183. 10.1023/B:PREV.0000037640.66607.6b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Doran N, & Trim RS (2015). Correlates of other tobacco use in a community sample of young adults. Addictive Behaviors, 51, 131–135. 10.1016/j.addbeh.2015.07.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Erickson DJ, Lenk KM, & Forster JL (2014). Latent classes of young adults based on use of multiple types of tobacco and nicotine products. Nicotine and Tobacco Research, 16, 1056–1062. 10.1093/ntr/ntu024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Evans-Polce R, Lanza S, & Maggs J (2016). Heterogeneity of alcohol, tobacco, and other substance use behaviors in U.S. college students: A latent class analysis. Addictive Behaviors, 53, 80–85. 10.1016/j.addbeh.2015.10.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Eysenck HJ (2012). Smoking, personality, and stress: Psychosocial factors in the prevention of cancer and coronary heart disease. Springer-Verlag New York Inc. [Google Scholar]
  29. Foreyt JP, Jackson AS, Squires WG Jr., Hartung GH, Murray TD, & Gotto AM Jr. (1993). Psychological profile of college students who use smokeless tobacco. Addictive Behaviors, 18, 107–116. 10.1016/0306-4603(93)90040-G. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Gonzalez A, Zvolensky MJ, Vujanovic AA, Leyro TM, & Marshall EC (2008). An evaluation of anxiety sensitivity, emotional dysregulation, and negative affectivity among daily cigarette smokers: Relation to smoking motives and barriers to quitting. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 43, 138–147. 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2008.03.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Goodman E, Adler NE, Kawachi I, Frazier AL, Huang B, & Colditz GA (2001). Adolescents' perceptions of social status: Development and evaluation of a new indicator. Pediatrics, 108, E31. 10.1542/peds.108.2.e31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Hair JF, Ringle CM, & Sarstedt M (2011). PLS-SEM: Indeed a silver bullet. Journal of Marketing Theory and Practice, 19, 139–151. 10.2753/MTP1069-6679190202. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  33. Hampson SE, & Friedman HS (2008). Personality and health: A life span perspective. In John OP, Robins R, & Pervin L (Eds.). The handbook of personality(3rd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
  34. Harakeh Z, Scholte RHJ, de Vries H, & Engels RCME (2006). Association between personality and adolescent smoking. Addictive Behaviors, 31, 232–245. 10.1016/j.addbeh.2005.05.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Harrell PT, Naqvu SMH, Plunk AD, Ji M, & Martins SS (2017). Patterns of youth tobacco and polytobacco usage: The shift to alternative tobacco products. American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse, 43, 694–701. 10.1080/00952990.2016.1225072. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Hinds JT 3rd, Loukas A, Chow S, Pasch KE, Harrell MB, Perry CL, … Wackowski OA (2016). Using cognitive interviewing to better assess young adult e-cigarette use. Nicotine and Tobacco Research, 18, 1998–2005. 10.1093/ntr/ntw096. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Hofmann W, Friese M, & Strack F (2009). Impulse and self-control from a dual-systems perspective. Perspectives in Psychological Science, 4, 162–176. 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01116.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Hudson NW, & Fraley RC (2015). Volitional personality trait change: Can people choose to change their personality traits? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109, 490–507. 10.1037/pspp0000021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Krishnan-Sarin S, Jackson A, Morean M, Kong G, Bold KW, Camenga DR, … Wu R (2019). E-cigarette devices used by high-school youth. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 194, 395–400. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2018.10.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Lenk K, Rode P, Fabian L, Bernat D, Klein E, & Forster J (2012). Cigarette use among young adults: Comparisons between two-year college students, four-year college students, and those not in college. Journal of American College Health, 60, 303–308. 10.1080/07448481.2011.607481. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Leventhal AM, Strong DR, Sussman S, Kirkpatrick MG, Unger JB, Barrington-Trimis JL, & Audrain-McGovern J (2016). Psychiatric comorbidity in adolescent electronic and conventional cigarette use. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 73, 71–78. 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2015.11.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Ling PM, & Glantz SA (2002). Why and how the tobacco industry sells cigarettes to young adults: Evidence from industry documents. American Journal of Public Health, 92, 908–916. 10.2105/ajph.92.6.908. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Lipari RN, & Van Horn SL (2017). Trends in smokeless tobacco use and initiation: 2002 to 2014. The CBHSQ report. Rockville, MD: Center for Behavioral Health Statistics and Quality, Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Lipperman-Kreda S, Paschall MJ, Robert FS, & Morrison CN (2018). Places and social contexts associated with simultaneous use of alcohol, tobacco and marijuana among young adults. Drug and Alcohol Review, 37, 188–195. 10.1111/dar.12537. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Lisha NE, Thrul J, & Ling PM (2019). Latent class analysis to examine patterns of smoking and other tobacco products in young adult bar patrons. Journal of Adolescent Health, 64, 93–98. 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2018.06.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Lo Y, Mendell N, & Rubin D (2001). Testing the number of components in a normal mixture. Biometrika, 88, 767–778. 10.1093/biomet/88.3.767. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  47. Lynam DR, Smith GT, Whiteside SP, & Cyders MA (2006). The UPPS-P: Assessing five personality pathways to impulsive behavior. Technical ReportWest Lafayette: Purdue University. [Google Scholar]
  48. Malouff JM, Thorsteinsson EB, & Schutte NS (2006). The five-factors model of personality and smoking: A meta-analysis. Journal of Drug Education, 36, 47–58. 10.2190/9EP8-17P8-EKG7-66AD. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Mays D, Arrazola RA, Tworek C, Rolle IV, Neff LJ, & Portnoy DB (2016). Openness to using non-cigarette tobacco products among U.S. young adults. American Journal of Preventative Medicine, 50, 528–534. 10.1016/j.amepre.2015.08.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. McCrae RR, & Costa PT Jr. (1997). Personality trait structure as a human universal. American Psychologist, 52, 509–516. 10.1037/0003-066X.52.5.509. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Messer K, Vijavaraghavan M, White MM, Shi Y, Chang C, Conway KP, … Pierce JP (2015). Cigarette smoking cessation attempts among current US smokers who also use smokeless tobacco. Addictive Behaviors, 51, 113–119. 10.1016/j.addbeh.2015.06.045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Muthén LK, & Muthén BO (1998–2002). Mplus user's guide (7th ed.). (Los Angeles, California: ). [Google Scholar]
  53. Nylund KL (2007). Latent transition analysis: Modeling extensions and an application to peer victimization. Doctoral dissertation Los Angeles: University of California. Retrieved from http://statmodel.com/papers.shtml. [Google Scholar]
  54. Nylund KL, Asparouhov T, & Muthén BO (2007). Deciding on the number of classes in latent class analysis and growth mixture modeling: A Monte Carlo simulation study. Structural Equation Modeling, 14, 535–569. 10.1080/10705510701575396. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  55. Odum AL (2011). Delay discounting: I'm a k, you're a k. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 96, 427–439. 10.1901/jeab.2011.96-423. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Phillips E, Wang TW, Husten CG, Corey CG, Apelberg BJ, Jamal A, … King BA (2017). Tobacco product use among adults – United States, 2015. MMWR Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 66, 1209–1215. 10.15585/mmwr.mm6644a2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Pulvers K, Ridenour C, Woodcock A, Savin MJ, Holguin G, Hamill S, & Romero DR (2018). Marijuana use among adolescent multiple tobacco product users and unique risks of dual tobacco and marijuana use. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 189, 80–89. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2018.04.031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Roberts BW, & Jackson JJ (2008). Sociogenomic personality psychology. Journal of Personality, 76, 1523–1544. 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2008.00530.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Roberts BW, Luo J, Briley DA, Chow PE, Su R, & Hill PL (2017). A systematic review of personality change through intervention. Psychological Bulletin, 143, 117–141. 10.1037/bul0000088. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Sharapova S, Reyes-Guzman C, Singh T, Phillips E, Marynak KL, & Agaku I (2018). Age of tobacco use initiation and association with current use and nicotine dependence among US middle and high school students, 2014–2016. Tobacco Control. 10.1136/tobaccocontrol-2018-054593 pii. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Shavers VL (2007). Measurement of socioeconomic status in health disparities research. Journal of the National Medical Association, 99, 1013–1023. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Simon P, Camenga DR, Kong G, Connell CM, Morean ME, Cavallo DA, & Krishnan-Sarin S (2017). Youth e-cigarette, blunt, and other tobacco use profiles: Does SES matter? Tobacco Regulatory Science, 3, 115–127. 10.18001/TRS.3.1.12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Slade T, Chapman C, Swift W, Keyes K, Tonks Z, &Teesson M (2016). Birth cohort trends in the global epidemiology of alcohol use and alcohol-related harms in men and women: Systematic review and metaregression. BMJ Open, 6, e011827. 10.1136/bmjopen-2016-011827. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Soneji S, Sargent J, & Tanski S (2016). Multiple tobacco product use among US adolescents and young adults. Tobacco Control 25, 174–180. 10.1136/tobaccocontrol-2014-051638. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Soto CJ, & John OP (2017). The next Big Five Inventory (BFI02): Developing and assessing a hierarchical model with 15 facets to enhance bandwidth, fidelity, and predictive power. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 113, 117–143. 10.1037/pspp0000096. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Stein JS, Wilson AG, Koffarnus MN, Daniel TO, Epstein LH, & Bickel WK (2016). Unstuck in time: Episodic future thinking reduces delay discounting and cigarette smoking. Psychopharmacology, 233, 3771–3778. 10.1007/s00213-016-4410-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration (SAMHSA) (2017). National survey on drug use and health: Detailed tables external. Substance abuse and mental health data archive.
  68. Svedberg P, Nygren JM, Staland-Nyman C, & Nyholm M (2016). The validity of socioeconomic status measures among adolescents based on self-reported information about parents occupations, FAS and perceived SES; implication for health related quality of life studies. BMC Medical Research Methodology, 16, 48. 10.1186/s12874-016-0148-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Terracciano A, & Costa PT Jr. (2004). Smoking and the five-factor model of personality. Addiction, 99, 472–481. 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2004.00687.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Turiano NA, Chapman BP, Gruenewald TL, & Mroczek DK (2015). Personality and the leading behavioral contributors of mortality. Health Psychology, 34, 51–60. 10.1037/hea0000038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Turiano NA, Whiteman SD, Hampson SE, Roberts BW, & Mroczek DK (2012). Personality and substance use in midlife: Conscientiousness as a moderator and the effects of trait change. Journal of Research on Personality, 46, 295–305. 10.1016/j.jrp.2012.02.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Um M, Hershberger AR, Whitt ZT, & Cyders MA (2018). Recommendations for applying a multi-dimensional model of impulsive personality to diagnosis and treatment. Borderline Personality Disorders and Emotion Dysregulation, 5, 6. 10.1186/S40479-018-0084-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Vuong Q (1989). Likelihood ratio tests for model selection and non-tested hypotheses. Econometrica, 57, 307–333. 10.2307/1912557. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  74. Wang TW, Asman K, Gentzke AS, Cullen KA, Holder-Hayes E, Reyes-Guzman C, … King BA (2018). Tobacco product use among adults – United States, 2017. MMWR Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 67, 1225–1232. 10.15585/mmwr.mm6744a2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Wellman RJ, Dugas EN, Dutczak H, O'Loughlin EK, Datta GD, Lauzon B, & O'Loughlin J (2016). Predictors of the onset of cigarette smoking: A systematic review of longitudinal population-based studies in youth. American Journal of Preventative Medicine, 51, 767–778. 10.1016/j.amepre.2016.04.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Wellman RJ, Sylvestre MP, O'Loughlin EK, Dutczak H, Montreuil A, Datta GD, & O'Loughlin J (2018). Socioeconomic status is associated with the prevalence and co-occurrence of risk factors for cigarette smoking initiation during adolescence. International Journal of Public Health, 63, 125–236. 10.1007/s00038-017-1051-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. West JC, Villanti AC, Graham AL, Mays D, Mermelstein RJ, & Higgins ST (2019). Tobacco use and cessation behaviors in young adults: 2016 National Health Interview Survey. American Journal of Public Health, 109, 296–299. 10.2105/AJPH.2018.304815. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. West Virginia Department of Health and Human Resources, Bureau for Public Health, Health Statistics Center (2015). Tobacco use prevalence among youth. West Virginia Youth Tobacco Survey2013.
  79. Yu M, Sacco P, Choi HJ, & Wintemberg J (2018). Identifying patterns of tobacco use among US middle and high school students: A latent class analysis. Addictive Behaviors, 79, 1–7. 10.1016/j.addbeh.2017.11.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Yuan M, Cross SJ, Loughlin SE, & Leslie FM (2015). Nicotine and the adolescent brain. Journal of Physiology, 593, 3397–3412. 10.1113/JP270492. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Zvolensky MJ, Taha F, Bono A, & Goodwin RD (2015). Big five personality factors and cigarette smoking: A 10-year study among US adults. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 63, 91–96. 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2015.02.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES