iBet uBet web content aggregator. Adding the entire web to your favor.
iBet uBet web content aggregator. Adding the entire web to your favor.



Link to original content: http://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/16596975/
On aims and methods in the neuroimaging of derived relations - PubMed Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
Review
. 2005 Nov;84(3):453-83.
doi: 10.1901/jeab.2005.92-04.

On aims and methods in the neuroimaging of derived relations

Affiliations
Review

On aims and methods in the neuroimaging of derived relations

David W Dickins. J Exp Anal Behav. 2005 Nov.

Abstract

Ingenious and seemingly powerful technologies have been developed recently that enable the visualization in some detail of events in the brain concomitant upon the ongoing behavioral performance of a human participant. Measurement of such brain events offers at the very least a new set of dependent variables in relation to which the independent variables familiarly manipulated in the operant laboratory may be explored. Two related paradigms in which a start has been made in such research concern the derivation of novel or emergent relations from a baseline set of trained relations, and include the phenomenon of transitive inference (TI), observed in studies of stimulus equivalence (SE) and serial learning (SL) or seriation. This paper reviews some published and forthcoming neuroimaging studies of these and related phenomena, and considers how this line of research both demands and represents a welcome synthesis between types of question and levels of explanation in behavioral science that often have been seen as antithetical.

PubMed Disclaimer

Figures

Figure 1
Figure 1. Top: Serial position effect in serial learning.
Latencies of trained responses at the end of training in serial learning as a function of the position of the stimuli on the implicit series in an instructed versus an uninstructed group of participants. Bottom: Symbolic distance effect in serial learning. Latencies of transitive responses after serial learning as a function of number of intervening stimuli in an instructed versus an uninstructed group of participants. (Modified from Anderson & Dickins, 2003).
Figure 2
Figure 2. Timelines for arbitrary MTS compared with identity matching with zero and nonzero delays (D. W. Dickins, 2003) (not drawn to any particular scale).
Upper lines: onset and offset of sample stimulus (open) and comparison stimuli (hatched), with or without an interstimulus interval (nonzero or zero delay). Response latency is measured from the onset of the comparison stimuli until the response. Lower boxes: postulated succession of hypothetical component processes. Key: 1  =  registration, 2  =  retrieval of trained relation, 3  =  scan of comparisons, 4  =  recognition of correct comparison, 5  =  organisation of response; black unnumbered box indicates no postulated process occurring. See text for explanation.
Figure 3
Figure 3. Effects of delay on identity matching and arbitrary (i.e., symbolic) MTS tasks.
Shown are the mean of the median response latencies after zero and nonzero delays between sample offset and onset of the six comparison stimuli.
Figure 4
Figure 4. Schematic representation of shared and specific features of the three types of matching-to-sample tasks employed in D. W. Dickins et al. (2001).
The results of this fMRI investigation conformed more closely with (b). (a) An additive hierarchy in which the upper tasks encompass all of the features of those below them, plus specific features of their own. (b) A Venn diagram in which each task shares some features with one and some with both of the other tasks, but also has its own peculiar features.
Figure 5
Figure 5. Glass brains showing significant differences when activations obtained on various control and experimental tasks were compared (Dickins et al., 2002.
On the right of each pair is a transverse section, and on the left is a saggital view of a transparent brain, with the anterior pole to the right. Dark areas indicate significant clusters of voxels, and the salient ones are labelled with their Brodmann areas. (a) full identity matching task minus asterisk control task; (b) trained relations task minus full identity task; (c) full identity task minus trained relations task; and (d) trained relations task minus asterisk control task.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Acuna B.D, Eliassen J.C, Donoghue J.P, Sanes J.N. Frontal and parietal lobe activation during transitive inference in humans. Cerebral Cortex. 2002;12:1312–1321. - PubMed
    1. Acuna B.D, Sanes J.N, Donoghue J.P. Cognitive mechanisms of transitive inference. Experimental Brain Research. 2002;146:1–10. - PubMed
    1. Anderson C, Dickins D.W. ‘Nodal distance’ versus ‘symbolic distance’: A contrast between two relational frames. Parma, Italy: 2003. Paper presented at the meeting of the European Association for Behavioural Analysis,
    1. Beason-Held L.L, Purpura K.P, Van Meter J.W, Azari N.P, Mangot D.J, Optican L.M, Mentis M.J, Alexander G.E, Grady C.L, Horwitz B, Rapoport S.I, Schapiro M.B. PET reveals occipitotemporal pathway activation during elementary form perception in humans. Visual Neuroscience. 1998;15:503–510. - PubMed
    1. Bentall R.P, Dickins D.W, Fox S.R.A. Naming and equivalence: Response latencies for emergent relations. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Comparative and Physiological Psychology. 1993;46B:187–214.

MeSH terms